Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 1 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 1 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 4 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 12 |
Descriptor
Nonverbal Communication | 14 |
Object Manipulation | 14 |
Imitation | 3 |
Infants | 3 |
Interaction | 3 |
Problem Solving | 3 |
Spatial Ability | 3 |
Age Differences | 2 |
Children | 2 |
Individual Differences | 2 |
Instructional Effectiveness | 2 |
More ▼ |
Source
Author
Bartlett, James C. | 1 |
Bolton, Sharon | 1 |
Caselli, Maria Cristina | 1 |
Chainay, H. | 1 |
Chu, Mingyuan | 1 |
Eliza L. Congdon | 1 |
Foley, Alana E. | 1 |
Goldin-Meadow, Susan | 1 |
Graham, Teresa K. | 1 |
Gutmann, Arlyne J. | 1 |
Humphreys, G. W. | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 12 |
Reports - Research | 11 |
Dissertations/Theses -… | 1 |
Guides - Non-Classroom | 1 |
Reports - Evaluative | 1 |
Education Level
Early Childhood Education | 2 |
Elementary Education | 1 |
Grade 1 | 1 |
Higher Education | 1 |
Postsecondary Education | 1 |
Primary Education | 1 |
Audience
Parents | 1 |
Location
Italy | 1 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
Autism Diagnostic Observation… | 1 |
MacArthur Communicative… | 1 |
Mullen Scales of Early… | 1 |
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Eliza L. Congdon – Child Development, 2024
Why is instructional gesture ineffective in some contexts? And what is it about learners that predicts whether they will learn from gestures? This between-subjects linear measurement training study compares gesture instruction to two controls--operant action and transient action--in a diverse sample of first-grade students (N = 174, M[subscript…
Descriptors: Individual Differences, Short Term Memory, Nonverbal Communication, Grade 1
Wakefield, Elizabeth M.; Foley, Alana E.; Ping, Raedy; Villarreal, Julia N.; Goldin-Meadow, Susan; Levine, Susan C. – Developmental Psychology, 2019
Past research has shown that children's mental rotation skills are malleable and can be improved through action experience--physically rotating objects--or gesture experience--showing how objects could rotate (e.g., Frick, Ferrara, & Newcombe, 2013; Goldin-Meadow et al., 2012; Levine, Goldin-Meadow, Carlson, & Hemani-Lopez, 2018). These…
Descriptors: Spatial Ability, Visualization, Nonverbal Communication, Motion
West, Kelsey L.; Roemer, Emily J.; Northrup, Jessie B.; Iverson, Jana M. – Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 2020
Purpose: Infants with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) produce fewer play actions and gestures than neurotypical infants (e.g., Mastrogiuseppe et al., 2015; Veness et al., 2012; Zwaigenbaum et al., 2005). The purpose of this study was to investigate whether different "types" of actions and gestures are more or less likely to develop…
Descriptors: Infants, Siblings, Autism, Pervasive Developmental Disorders
Stieff, Mike; Lira, Matthew E.; Scopelitis, Stephanie A. – Cognition and Instruction, 2016
The present article describes two studies that examine the impact of teaching students to use gesture to support spatial thinking in the Science, Technology, Engineering, and Mathematics (STEM) discipline of chemistry. In Study 1 we compared the effectiveness of instruction that involved either watching gesture, reproducing gesture, or reading…
Descriptors: Undergraduate Students, Organic Chemistry, STEM Education, Research Universities
Segal, Ayelet – ProQuest LLC, 2011
Can action support cognition? Can direct touch support performance? Embodied interaction involving digital devices is based on the theory of grounded cognition. Embodied interaction with gestural interfaces involves more of our senses than traditional (mouse-based) interfaces, and in particular includes direct touch and physical movement, which…
Descriptors: Evidence, Interaction, Arithmetic, Nonverbal Communication
Caselli, Maria Cristina; Rinaldi, Pasquale; Stefanini, Silvia; Volterra, Virginia – Child Development, 2012
Data from 492 Italian infants (8-18 months) were collected with the parental questionnaire MacArthur Bates Communicative Development Inventories to describe early actions and gestures (A-G) "vocabulary" and its relation with spoken vocabulary in both comprehension and production. A-G were more strongly correlated with word comprehension…
Descriptors: Oral Language, Object Manipulation, Language Acquisition, Vocabulary
Vauclair, Jacques; Imbault, Juliette – Developmental Science, 2009
The aim of this study was to measure the pattern of hand preferences for pointing gestures as a function of object-manipulation handedness in 123 infants and toddlers (10-40 months). The results showed that not only right-handers but also left-handers and ambidextrous participants tended to use their right hand for pointing. There was a…
Descriptors: Toddlers, Infants, Nonverbal Communication, Handedness
Itakura, Shoji; Ishida, Hiraku; Kanda, Takayuki; Shimada, Yohko; Ishiguro, Hiroshi; Lee, Kang – Infancy, 2008
This study examined whether young children are able to imitate a robot's goal-directed actions. Children (24-35 months old) viewed videos showing a robot attempting to manipulate an object (e.g., putting beads inside a cup) but failing to achieve its goal (e.g., beads fell outside the cup). In 1 video, the robot made eye contact with a human…
Descriptors: Imitation, Toddlers, Robotics, Nonverbal Communication
Chu, Mingyuan; Kita, Sotaro – Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 2008
This study investigated the motor strategy involved in mental rotation tasks by examining 2 types of spontaneous gestures (hand-object interaction gestures, representing the agentive hand action on an object, vs. object-movement gestures, representing the movement of an object by itself) and different types of verbal descriptions of rotation.…
Descriptors: Interaction, Spatial Ability, Nonverbal Communication, Cognitive Processes
Graham, Teresa K.; Scudder, Rosalind R. – Communication Disorders Quarterly, 2007
Language and social interactions were examined between a father and mother and their twin children, one with Down syndrome and one developing typically. Systematic observations were made of videotaped segments and coded in real time using a computerized behavior coding program. Codes for the parents' interactions consisted of total vocalizations,…
Descriptors: Twins, Down Syndrome, Speech Language Pathology, Object Manipulation
Chainay, H.; Louarn, C.; Humphreys, G. W. – Brain and Cognition, 2006
We report data from a group of patients with mild Alzheimer's disease on a range of tasks requiring either stored semantic knowledge about objects (e.g., naming object use) or the execution of action to objects (e.g., miming and using objects). We found that the patients were impaired at miming in response to objects, even when they could describe…
Descriptors: Semantics, Patients, Alzheimers Disease, Nonverbal Communication

Gutmann, Arlyne J.; Turnure, James E. – Developmental Psychology, 1979
Descriptors: Age Differences, Body Language, Mothers, Nonverbal Communication
Jones, Todd C.; Bartlett, James C.; Wade, Kimberley A. – Journal of Memory and Language, 2006
Conjunction errors occur when participants incorrectly identify as "old" novel test stimuli created by recombining parts of two study stimuli (parent items). Prior studies have reported that the conjunction error rate is higher when parent items are studied together than when they are studied apart (a parent proximity effect). In several…
Descriptors: Nonverbal Communication, Form Classes (Languages), Recognition (Psychology), Familiarity
Bolton, Sharon; Williamson, Kris Strom, Ed. – 1990
This booklet presents child-rearing practices found successful by families of children who are deaf-blind, and includes photographs of deaf-blind children using the techniques. Along with a broad overview of communications theory as it applies to young deaf-blind children, techniques are described for using tactile objects as keys to…
Descriptors: Communication Skills, Daily Living Skills, Deaf Blind, Motor Development