NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Showing all 14 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Riviere, James; David, Elodie – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2013
In the C-not-B task, 2.5-year-old children tend to look for an object in a location to which the hiding agent moved his hand (C) after moving an object from A to B. In three experiments, the authors investigated the nature of the constraints underlying toddlers' performance in this task. In Experiment 1, 2.5-year-olds were tested in a new version…
Descriptors: Young Children, Object Permanence, Toddlers, Experiments
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Zosh, Jennifer M.; Feigenson, Lisa – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2012
Accurate representation of a changing environment requires individuation--the ability to determine how many numerically distinct objects are present in a scene. Much research has characterized early individuation abilities by identifying which object features infants can use to individuate throughout development. However, despite the fact that…
Descriptors: Infants, Short Term Memory, Cognitive Ability, Task Analysis
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Beck, Sarah R.; McColgan, Kerry L. T.; Robinson, Elizabeth J.; Rowley, Martin G. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2011
Children's well-documented tendency to behave as if they know more than they do about uncertain events is reduced under two conditions: when the outcome of a chance event has yet to be determined and when one unknown outcome has occurred but is difficult to imagine. In Experiment 1, in line with published findings, 5- and 6-year-olds (N=61)…
Descriptors: Object Permanence, Experimental Psychology, Student Behavior, Identification
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Haddad, Jeffrey M.; Chen, Yuping; Keen, Rachel – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2011
The issue of whether young children use spatio-temporal information (e.g., movement of objects through time and space) and/or contact-mechanical information (e.g., interaction between objects) to search for a hidden object was investigated. To determine whether one cue can have priority over the other, a dynamic event that put these cues into…
Descriptors: Object Permanence, Cues, Eye Movements, Young Children
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Schum, Nina; Franz, Volker H.; Jovanovic, Bianca; Schwarzer, Gudrun – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2012
We investigated whether 6- and 7-year-olds and 9- and 10-year-olds, as well as adults, process object dimensions independent of or in interaction with one another in a perception and action task by adapting Ganel and Goodale's method for testing adults ("Nature", 2003, Vol. 426, pp. 664-667). In addition, we aimed to confirm Ganel and Goodale's…
Descriptors: Evidence, Handicrafts, Visual Perception, Interaction
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Moher, Mariko; Tuerk, Arin S.; Feigenson, Lisa – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2012
Although working memory has a highly constrained capacity limit of three or four items, both adults and toddlers can increase the total amount of stored information by "chunking" object representations in memory. To examine the developmental origins of chunking, we used a violation-of-expectation procedure to ask whether 7-month-old infants, whose…
Descriptors: Object Permanence, Cues, Infants, Short Term Memory
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Riggs, Kevin J.; Simpson, Andrew; Potts, Thomas – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2011
Visual short-term memory (VSTM) research suggests that the adult capacity is limited to three or four multifeature object representations. Despite evidence supporting a developmental increase in capacity, it remains unclear what the unit of capacity is in children. The current study employed the change detection paradigm to investigate both the…
Descriptors: Object Permanence, Short Term Memory, Recall (Psychology), Memorization
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Perry, Lynn K.; Samuelson, Larissa K.; Spencer, John P. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2009
This study investigated how young children's increasingly flexible use of spatial reference frames enables accurate search for hidden objects by using a task that 3-year-olds have been shown to perform with great accuracy and 2-year-olds have been shown to perform inaccurately. Children watched as an object was rolled down a ramp, behind a panel…
Descriptors: Object Permanence, Preschool Children, Task Analysis, Experiments
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Rakison, D.H. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2005
Four experiments examined the role of correlations between dynamic and static parts on 12- to 16-month-olds' ability to learn the identity of agents and recipients in a simple causal event. Infants were habituated to events in which objects with a dynamic or static part acted as an agent or a recipient and then were tested with an event in which…
Descriptors: Infants, Geometric Concepts, Cues, Object Permanence
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Butterworth, George – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1977
This study tested Piaget's explanation of infants' perseverative error in manual search by comparing searches when the object was (1) hidden, (2) covered but visible, and (3) uncovered. Errors occurred under all three conditions, with conflict at a maximum when the object was hidden. (Author/JMB)
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Egocentrism, Error Patterns, Infants
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
And Others; Moore, M. Keith – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1978
Descriptors: Age Differences, Conservation (Concept), Developmental Stages, Infant Behavior
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Diedrich, Frederick J.; Highlands, Tonia M.; Spahr, Kimberly A.; Thelen, Esther; Smith, Linda B. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2001
Evaluated in three experiments a dynamic systems theory account of perseverative errors on "A-not-B" task. Found that 9-month-olds perseverated when reaching for identical targets, but made nonperseverative responses when reaching in the presence of a highly distinctive B target. Reach direction was jointly determined by target's…
Descriptors: Cognitive Development, Cues, Error Patterns, Infant Behavior
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Sophian, Catherine; Wellman, Henry M. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1983
Experiment one tested 9- and 16-month-old children on a modification of Piaget's Stage IV object permanence task, examining infants' use of information from previous experiences with an object and from a recent hiding to locate a hidden object. In experiment two, two-, two-and-a-half-, and four-year-old children additionally received verbal…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Cognitive Ability, Cognitive Development, Cognitive Processes
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Ackerman, Brian P.; McGraw, Megan – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1991
Children and adults listened to stories that contained a goal sentence and an inconsistent outcome. Several encoding and retrieval factors were simultaneously manipulated in the stories. Interaction effects in situations with weak encoding constraint decreased with age. (BC)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Cognitive Development, College Students, Cues