NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Showing all 10 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Saksida, Amanda; Langus, Alan; Nespor, Marina – Developmental Science, 2017
To what extent can language acquisition be explained in terms of different associative learning mechanisms? It has been hypothesized that distributional regularities in spoken languages are strong enough to elicit statistical learning about dependencies among speech units. Distributional regularities could be a useful cue for word learning even…
Descriptors: Language Acquisition, Associative Learning, Cues, Oral Language
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
De Clerck, Ilke; Pettinato, Michele; Verhoeven, Jo; Gillis, Steven – Journal of Child Language, 2017
This study investigated the relation between lexical development and the production of prosodic prominence in disyllabic babble and words. Monthly recordings from nine typically developing Belgian-Dutch-speaking infants were analyzed from the onset of babbling until a cumulative vocabulary of 200 words was reached. The differentiation between the…
Descriptors: Longitudinal Studies, Language Acquisition, Child Language, Vocabulary Development
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Grunloh, Thomas; Liszkowski, Ulf – Journal of Child Language, 2015
The current study investigated whether point-accompanying characteristics, like vocalizations and hand shape, differentiate infants' underlying motives of prelinguistic pointing. We elicited imperative (requestive) and declarative (expressive and informative) pointing acts in experimentally controlled situations, and analyzed accompanying…
Descriptors: Child Language, Nonverbal Communication, Infants, Oral Language
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Burnett, Debra L. – Journal of Child Language, 2015
Irony comprehension in seven- and eight-year-old children with typically developing language skills was explored under the framework of the graded salience hypothesis. Target ironic remarks, either conventional or novel/situation-specific, were presented following brief story contexts. Children's responses to comprehension questions were used to…
Descriptors: Child Language, Young Children, Figurative Language, Comprehension
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Vernice, Mirta; Guasti, Maria Teresa – First Language, 2014
It remains controversial whether children are able to process and integrate specific linguistic cues in their mental model to the same extent as adults. In the present study, a sentence continuation task was employed to determine how Italian speakers (4-, 5-, 6-year-olds and adults) interpret prosodic cues to decide which referent is more salient…
Descriptors: Intonation, Suprasegmentals, Child Language, Language Acquisition
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Iyer, Suneeti Nathani; Oller, D. Kimbrough – Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 2008
Little research has been conducted on the development of suprasegmental characteristics of vocalizations in typically developing infants (TDI) and the role of audition in the development of these characteristics. The purpose of the present study was to examine the longitudinal development of fundamental frequency (F[subscript 0]) in eight TDI and…
Descriptors: Suprasegmentals, Hearing (Physiology), Infants, Hearing Impairments
Cruttenden, Alan – 1982
The evidence on the acquisition of intonation by children is reviewed. Reports on the early use of pitch contours fall into two categories, imitational and differential intonation. While imitational intonation is based on mimicry of adults, differential intonation involves the acquisition of two or three tunes that contrast in meaning from an…
Descriptors: Child Language, Intonation, Language Acquisition, Oral Language
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Marcos, Haydee – Journal of Child Language, 1987
Investigation of the communicative functions of pitch direction and range in one-year-olds (N=2) indicated that use of pitch among infants may be related to a period where communicative intentions are clearly defined, but language is not yet available. A higher pitch was observed among infants who made repeated requests for objects as opposed to…
Descriptors: Child Language, Communication Skills, Infants, Intonation
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Vogel, Irene; Raimy, Eric – Journal of Child Language, 2002
This paper investigates the acquisition of compound vs. phrasal stress ("hot dog" vs. "hot dog") in English. This has previously been shown to be acquired quite late, in contrast to recent research showing that infants both perceive and prefer rhythmic patterns in their own language. Subjects (40 children in four groups the averages ages of which…
Descriptors: Child Language, Foreign Countries, Phonology, Pronunciation
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Camarata, Stephen M.; Erwin, Lisa – Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 1988
A case study is presented in which a language impaired three-year-old used suprasegmental features to distinguish singular and plural forms in spontaneous speech. Acoustic analyses revealed that the suprasegmentals included various duration, fundamental frequency, and intensity parameters. Phonological, morphological, and psycholinguistic factors…
Descriptors: Acoustic Phonetics, Case Studies, Child Language, Language Handicaps