Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 3 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 10 |
Descriptor
Models | 13 |
Orthographic Symbols | 13 |
Alphabets | 11 |
Word Recognition | 7 |
English | 4 |
Phonology | 4 |
Cognitive Processes | 3 |
Experiments | 3 |
Language Processing | 3 |
Phonemes | 3 |
Reaction Time | 3 |
More ▼ |
Source
Cognitive Science | 2 |
Journal of Experimental… | 2 |
ProQuest LLC | 2 |
Psychological Review | 2 |
Grantee Submission | 1 |
Journal of Experimental Child… | 1 |
Journal of Memory and Language | 1 |
Online Submission | 1 |
Reading Teacher | 1 |
Author
Besner, Derek | 1 |
Chase, Christopher H. | 1 |
Christopher R. Cox | 1 |
Coltheart, Max | 1 |
Evers, Amy J. | 1 |
Fleri, Amanda M. | 1 |
Havelka, Jelena | 1 |
Johnson, Rebecca L. | 1 |
Kim, Tae Eun | 1 |
Kinoshita, Sachiko | 1 |
Lang, Lisa F. | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 9 |
Reports - Research | 6 |
Dissertations/Theses -… | 2 |
Reports - Descriptive | 2 |
Reports - Evaluative | 2 |
Information Analyses | 1 |
Speeches/Meeting Papers | 1 |
Education Level
Higher Education | 2 |
Adult Education | 1 |
Elementary Education | 1 |
Grade 1 | 1 |
Grade 5 | 1 |
Grade 8 | 1 |
Middle Schools | 1 |
Audience
Location
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Luthra, Sahil; You, Heejo; Rueckl, Jay G.; Magnuson, James S. – Cognitive Science, 2020
Visual word recognition is facilitated by the presence of "orthographic neighbors" that mismatch the target word by a single letter substitution. However, researchers typically do not consider "where" neighbors mismatch the target. In light of evidence that some letter positions are more informative than others, we investigate…
Descriptors: Visual Stimuli, Word Recognition, Orthographic Symbols, Alphabets
Christopher R. Cox; Matthew J. Cooper Borkenhagen; Mark S. Seidenberg – Grantee Submission, 2019
Learning to read English requires learning the complex statistical dependencies between orthography and phonology. Previous research has focused on how these statistics are learned in neural network models provided with as much training as needed. Children, however, are expected to acquire this knowledge in a few years of school with only limited…
Descriptors: Second Language Learning, English (Second Language), Reading Instruction, Orthographic Symbols
Testolin, Alberto; Stoianov, Ivilin; Sperduti, Alessandro; Zorzi, Marco – Cognitive Science, 2016
Learning the structure of event sequences is a ubiquitous problem in cognition and particularly in language. One possible solution is to learn a probabilistic generative model of sequences that allows making predictions about upcoming events. Though appealing from a neurobiological standpoint, this approach is typically not pursued in…
Descriptors: Orthographic Symbols, Neurological Organization, Models, Probability
Whitney, Carol; Marton, Yuval – Online Submission, 2013
The SERIOL model of orthographic analysis proposed mechanisms for converting visual input into a serial encoding of letter order, which involved hemisphere-specific processing at the retinotopic level. As a test of SERIOL predictions, we conducted a consonant trigram-identification experiment, where the trigrams were briefly presented at various…
Descriptors: Visual Stimuli, Word Recognition, Models, Orthographic Symbols
Reddy, Sravana – ProQuest LLC, 2012
The pronunciation of a word represented in an alphabetic writing system (such as this one) is relatively transparent--but a language's sounds change over time and vary across space, while its spellings remain relatively static, resulting in some amount of divergence between the written and spoken forms. The introduction of loanwords and…
Descriptors: Pronunciation, Alphabets, Phonology, Orthographic Symbols
Kim, Tae Eun – ProQuest LLC, 2012
This dissertation is about Chinese loanwords. It is mainly divided into two parts. Part I is a general discussion about loanwords in Chinese; Chapter I and II belong to the first part. Part II is a discussion about the analyses of Mandarin loanwords originating from English. Chapter III, IV, and V are all related to the second part. Chapter VI is…
Descriptors: Mandarin Chinese, Linguistic Borrowing, English, Japanese
Johnson, Rebecca L.; Staub, Adrian; Fleri, Amanda M. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2012
Printed words that have a transposed-letter (TL) neighbor (e.g., angel has the TL neighbor angle) have been shown to be more difficult to process, in a range of paradigms, than words that do not have a TL neighbor. However, eye movement evidence suggests that this processing difficulty may occur on only a subset of trials. To investigate this…
Descriptors: Alphabets, Word Recognition, Language Processing, Orthographic Symbols
Norris, Dennis; Kinoshita, Sachiko – Psychological Review, 2012
The goal of research on how letter identity and order are perceived during reading is often characterized as one of "cracking the orthographic code." Here, we suggest that there is no orthographic code to crack: Words are perceived and represented as sequences of letters, just as in a dictionary. Indeed, words are perceived and represented in…
Descriptors: Psychology, Research, Perception, Identification
Rastle, Kathleen; Havelka, Jelena; Wydell, Taeko N.; Coltheart, Max; Besner, Derek – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2009
The interaction between length and lexical status is one of the key findings used in support of models of reading aloud that postulate a serial process in the orthography-to-phonology translation (B. S. Weekes, 1997). However, proponents of parallel models argue that this effect arises in peripheral visual or articulatory processes. The authors…
Descriptors: Stimuli, Phonology, Alphabets, Orthographic Symbols
Evers, Amy J.; Lang, Lisa F.; Smith, Sharon V. – Reading Teacher, 2009
The authors describe how alphabet books teach so much more than just the ABCs. They provide excellent resources, allowing teachers to link and integrate the reciprocal processes of reading and writing. Encapsulated within the writing workshop framework, the authors use multigenre and multicultural alphabet books as anchor texts for a literacy…
Descriptors: Alphabets, Orthographic Symbols, Parent Participation, Writing Workshops

Paap, Kenneth R.; And Others – Psychological Review, 1982
An encoding algorithm uses empirically determined confusion matrices to activate units in an alphabetum and a lexicon to predict performance of word, orthographically regular nonword, or irregular nonword recognition. Performance is enhanced when decisions are based on lexical information which constrains test letter identity. Word prediction…
Descriptors: Letters (Alphabet), Lexicology, Models, Orthographic Symbols
Perea, Manuel; Lupker, Stephen J. – Journal of Memory and Language, 2004
Nonwords created by transposing two "adjacent" letters (i.e., transposed-letter (TL) nonwords like "jugde") are very effective at activating the lexical representation of their base words. This fact poses problems for most computational models of word recognition (e.g., the interactive-activation model and its extensions), which assume that exact…
Descriptors: Alphabets, Word Recognition, Models, Lexicology

Chase, Christopher H.; Tallal, Paula – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1990
Examined effects of orthographic context on the letter recognition skills of dyslexic children, comparing their performance to that of adults and of chronological and reading age-matched groups. Results showed that the two matched groups showed strong word superiority effect (WSE) for words and pseudowords over nonwords. Dyslexic readers did not…
Descriptors: Adults, Age Differences, Children, Cognitive Processes