NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Showing all 13 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Merck, Catherine; Jonin, Pierre-Yves; Vichard, Helene; Boursiquot, Sandrine Le Moal; Leblay, Virginie; Belliard, Serge – Brain and Language, 2013
Category-specific deficits have rarely been reported in semantic dementia (SD). To our knowledge, only four previous studies have documented category-specific deficits, and these have focused on the living versus non-living things contrast rather than on more fine-grained semantic categories. This study aimed to determine whether a…
Descriptors: Alzheimers Disease, Semantics, Patients, Food
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Knolle, Franziska; Schroger, Erich; Baess, Pamela; Kotz, Sonja A. – Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 2012
Forward predictions are crucial in motor action (e.g., catching a ball, or being tickled) but may also apply to sensory or cognitive processes (e.g., listening to distorted speech or to a foreign accent). According to the "internal forward model," the cerebellum generates predictions about somatosensory consequences of movements. These predictions…
Descriptors: Brain Hemisphere Functions, Cognitive Processes, Pronunciation, Evidence
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Reggev, Niv; Zuckerman, Maya; Maril, Anat – Neuropsychologia, 2011
Metamemory refers to the ability of individuals to monitor and control their own memory performance. Although little theoretical consideration of the possible differences between the monitoring of episodic and of semantic knowledge has been published, results from patient and drug studies that used the "feeling of knowing" (FOK) paradigm show a…
Descriptors: Semantics, Prediction, Metacognition, Memory
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Moustafa, Ahmed A.; Gluck, Mark A. – Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 2011
Most existing models of dopamine and learning in Parkinson disease (PD) focus on simulating the role of basal ganglia dopamine in reinforcement learning. Much data argue, however, for a critical role for prefrontal cortex (PFC) dopamine in stimulus selection in attentional learning. Here, we present a new computational model that simulates…
Descriptors: Neurology, Patients, Reinforcement, Cognitive Development
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Croft, Katie E.; Duff, Melissa C.; Kovach, Christopher K.; Anderson, Steven W.; Adolphs, Ralph; Tranel, Daniel – Neuropsychologia, 2010
As we learn new information about the social and moral behaviors of other people, we form and update character judgments of them, and this can profoundly influence how we regard and act towards others. In the study reported here, we capitalized on two interesting neurological patient populations where this process of complex "moral…
Descriptors: Neurology, Personality, Patients, Moral Values
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Protzner, Andrea B.; McAndrews, Mary Pat – Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 2011
Although the hippocampus is not considered a key structure in semantic memory, patients with medial-temporal lobe epilepsy (mTLE) have deficits in semantic access on some word retrieval tasks. We hypothesized that these deficits reflect the negative impact of focal epilepsy on remote cerebral structures. Thus, we expected that the networks that…
Descriptors: Epilepsy, Semantics, Verbs, Patients
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Gadea, Marien; Espert, Raul; Salvador, Alicia; Marti-Bonmati, Luis – Brain and Cognition, 2011
Dichotic Listening (DL) is a valuable tool to study emotional brain lateralization. Regarding the perception of sadness and anger through affective prosody, the main finding has been a left ear advantage (LEA) for the sad but contradictory data for the anger prosody. Regarding an induced mood in the laboratory, its consequences upon DL were a…
Descriptors: Cognitive Style, Brain Hemisphere Functions, Patients, School Districts
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Palminteri, Stefano; Lebreton, Mael; Worbe, Yulia; Hartmann, Andreas; Lehericy, Stephane; Vidailhet, Marie; Grabli, David; Pessiglione, Mathias – Brain, 2011
Reinforcement learning theory has been extensively used to understand the neural underpinnings of instrumental behaviour. A central assumption surrounds dopamine signalling reward prediction errors, so as to update action values and ensure better choices in the future. However, educators may share the intuitive idea that reinforcements not only…
Descriptors: Learning Theories, Models, Symptoms (Individual Disorders), Prediction
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Langguth, Berthold; Juttner, Martin; Landis, Theodor; Regard, Marianne; Rentschler, Ingo – Neuropsychologia, 2009
Hemispheric differences in the learning and generalization of pattern categories were explored in two experiments involving sixteen patients with unilateral posterior, cerebral lesions in the left (LH) or right (RH) hemisphere. In each experiment participants were first trained to criterion in a supervised learning paradigm to categorize a set of…
Descriptors: Patients, Classification, Geometric Concepts, Generalization
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Cavanagh, James F.; Grundler, Theo O. J.; Frank, Michael J.; Allen, John J. B. – Neuropsychologia, 2010
Larger error-related negativities (ERNs) have been consistently found in obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) patients, and are thought to reflect the activities of a hyperactive cortico-striatal circuit during action monitoring. We previously observed that obsessive-compulsive (OC) symptomatic students (non-patients) have larger ERNs during errors…
Descriptors: Competition, Patients, Memory, Anatomy
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Crosson, Bruce; Moore, Anna Bacon; McGregor, Keith M.; Chang, Yu-Ling; Benjamin, Michelle; Gopinath, Kaundinya; Sherod, Megan E.; Wierenga, Christina E.; Peck, Kyung K.; Briggs, Richard W.; Rothi, Leslie J. Gonzalez; White, Keith D. – Brain and Language, 2009
Five nonfluent aphasia patients participated in a picture-naming treatment that used an intention manipulation (opening a box and pressing a button on a device in the box with the left hand) to initiate naming trials and was designed to re-lateralize word production mechanisms from the left to the right frontal lobe. To test the underlying…
Descriptors: Aphasia, Patients, Attention Deficit Disorders, Brain Hemisphere Functions
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Kehagia, Angie A.; Cools, Roshan; Barker, Roger A.; Robbins, Trevor W. – Neuropsychologia, 2009
This study sought to disambiguate the impact of Parkinson's disease (PD) on cognitive control as indexed by task set switching, by addressing discrepancies in the literature pertaining to disease severity and paradigm heterogeneity. A task set is governed by a rule that determines how relevant stimuli (stimulus set) map onto specific responses…
Descriptors: Stimuli, Diseases, Pathology, Rating Scales
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Bloch, Michael H.; Sukhodolsky, Denis G.; Leckman, James F.; Schultz, Robert T. – Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 2006
Background: Most children with Tourette's syndrome (TS) experience a significant decline in tic symptoms during adolescence. Currently no clinical measures have been identified that can predict whose tic symptoms will persist into adulthood. Patients with TS have deficits on neuropsychological tests involving fine-motor coordination and…
Descriptors: Neurological Impairments, Economically Disadvantaged, Testing, Children