Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 0 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 5 |
Descriptor
Phonology | 6 |
Word Frequency | 6 |
Language Processing | 4 |
Models | 3 |
Semantics | 3 |
Phonemes | 2 |
Recall (Psychology) | 2 |
Aphasia | 1 |
Children | 1 |
Cognitive Processes | 1 |
Comparative Analysis | 1 |
More ▼ |
Source
Journal of Memory and Language | 6 |
Author
Allen, Richard | 1 |
Aue, William R. | 1 |
Balota, David A. | 1 |
Criss, Amy H. | 1 |
Dell, Gary S. | 1 |
Hulme, Charles | 1 |
J. Lipinski | 1 |
Kittredge, Audrey K. | 1 |
Nozari, Nazbanou | 1 |
P. Gupta | 1 |
Schwartz, Myrna F. | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 6 |
Reports - Research | 6 |
Education Level
Audience
Location
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Criss, Amy H.; Aue, William R.; Smith, Larissa – Journal of Memory and Language, 2011
Normative word frequency and context variability affect memory in a range of episodic memory tasks and place constraints on theoretical development. In four experiments, we independently manipulated the word frequency and context variability of the targets (to-be-generated items) and cues in a cued recall paradigm. We found that high frequency…
Descriptors: Cues, Memory, Word Frequency, Recall (Psychology)
Yap, Melvin J.; Balota, David A. – Journal of Memory and Language, 2009
The visual word recognition literature has been dominated by the study of "monosyllabic" words in factorial experiments, computational models, and megastudies. However, it is not yet clear whether the behavioral effects reported for monosyllabic words generalize reliably to "multisyllabic" words. Hierarchical regression techniques were used to…
Descriptors: Semantics, Word Recognition, Word Frequency, Models
Nozari, Nazbanou; Kittredge, Audrey K.; Dell, Gary S.; Schwartz, Myrna F. – Journal of Memory and Language, 2010
This paper investigates the cognitive processes underlying picture naming and auditory word repetition. In the two-step model of lexical access, both the semantic and phonological steps are involved in naming, but the former has no role in repetition. Assuming recognition of the to-be-repeated word, repetition could consist of retrieving the…
Descriptors: Phonemes, Phonology, Semantics, Aphasia
Stemberger, Joseph Paul – Journal of Memory and Language, 2007
Overtensing (the use of an inflected form in place of a nonfinite form, e.g. *"didn't broke" for target "didn't break") is common in early syntax. In a ChiLDES-based study of 36 children acquiring English, I examine the effects of phonological and lexical factors. For irregulars, errors are more common with verbs of low frequency and when…
Descriptors: Syntax, Rhyme, Morphemes, Error Patterns
J. Lipinski; P. Gupta – Journal of Memory and Language, 2005
Twelve experiments examined the effect of neighborhood density on repetition latency for nonwords. Previous reports have indicated that nonwords from high density neighborhoods are repeated with shorter latency than nonwords from low density neighborhoods (e.g., Vitevitch & Luce, 1998). Experiment 1 replicated these previously reported…
Descriptors: Word Frequency, Phonology, Repetition, Language Processing
Allen, Richard; Hulme, Charles – Journal of Memory and Language, 2006
We report two experiments examining the role of concreteness and word phonological neighborhood characteristics on immediate serial recall. In line with previous findings concreteness, word frequency, and larger neighborhood size are associated with better serial recall. Both concreteness and word neighborhood size were also positively associated…
Descriptors: Speech Communication, Language Processing, Recall (Psychology), Word Frequency