NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Showing 1 to 15 of 96 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Sarah C. Creel – Child Development, 2025
How does one assess developmental change when the measures themselves change with development? Most developmental studies of word learning use either looking (infants) or pointing (preschoolers and older). With little empirical evidence of the relationship between the two measures, developmental change is difficult to assess. This paper analyzes…
Descriptors: Preschool Children, Vocabulary Development, Language Acquisition, Accuracy
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Doherty, Martin J.; Wimmer, Marina C.; Gollek, Cornelia; Stone, Charlotte; Robinson, Elizabeth J. – Child Development, 2021
Jigsaw puzzles are ubiquitous developmental toys in Western societies, used here to examine the development of metarepresentation. For jigsaw puzzles this entails understanding that individual pieces, when assembled, produce a picture. In Experiment 1, 3- to 5-year-olds (N = 117) completed jigsaw puzzles that were normal, had no picture, or…
Descriptors: Puzzles, Metacognition, Cognitive Development, Young Children
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Cowan, Nelson – Child Development, 2021
The Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence, 4th ed. includes two measures of working memory normed on children 2;6-7;7. The present analyses of the typically developing children (N = 1,591, 812 female, 779 male, with an ethnic distribution approximating the United States) provide new, theoretically important information about these…
Descriptors: Cognitive Development, Intelligence Tests, Young Children, Short Term Memory
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Treiman, Rebecca; Hompluem, Lana; Gordon, Jessica; Decker, Kristina; Markson, Lori – Child Development, 2016
Two experiments with one hundred and fourteen 3- to 5-year-old children examined whether children understand that a printed word represents a specific spoken word and that it differs in this way from a drawing. When an experimenter read a word to children and then a puppet used a different but related label for it, such as "dog" for the…
Descriptors: Young Children, Puppetry, Comprehension, Written Language
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Marentette, Paula; Pettenati, Paola; Bello, Arianna; Volterra, Virginia – Child Development, 2016
Analyses of elicited pantomime, primarily of English-speaking children, show that preschool-aged children are more likely to symbolically represent an object with gestures depicting an object's form rather than its function. In contrast, anecdotal reports of spontaneous gesture production in younger children suggest that children use multiple…
Descriptors: Nonverbal Communication, Child Development, Italian, English
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Geurten, Marie; Lloyd, Marianne; Willems, Sylvie – Child Development, 2017
Previous research has suggested that fluency does not influence memory decisions until ages 7-8. In two experiments (n = 96 and n = 64, respectively), children, aged 4, 6, and 8 years (Experiments 1 and 2), and adults (Experiment 2) studied a list of pictures. Participants completed a recognition test during which each study item was preceded by a…
Descriptors: Language Fluency, Young Children, Children, Memory
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Bergelson, Elika; Swingley, Daniel – Child Development, 2018
To understand spoken words, listeners must appropriately interpret co-occurring talker characteristics and speech sound content. This ability was tested in 6- to 14-months-olds by measuring their looking to named food and body part images. In the "new talker" condition (n = 90), pictures were named by an unfamiliar voice; in the…
Descriptors: Infants, Language Processing, Infant Behavior, Food
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Lansu, Tessa A. M.; Cillessen, Antonius H. N.; Karremans, Johan C. – Child Development, 2014
Previous research has shown that adolescents' attention for a peer is determined by the peer's status. This study examined how it is also determined by the status of the perceiving adolescent, and the gender of both parties involved (perceiver and perceived). Participants were 122 early adolescents (M age = 11.0 years) who completed…
Descriptors: Peer Relationship, Eye Movements, Social Status, Early Adolescents
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Shinskey, Jeanne L.; Jachens, Liza J. – Child Development, 2014
Infants' transfer of information from pictures to objects was tested by familiarizing 9-month-olds (N = 31) with either a color or black-and-white photograph of an object and observing their preferential reaching for the real target object versus a distractor. One condition tested object recognition by keeping both objects visible, and the…
Descriptors: Infants, Child Development, Photography, Color
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Skoruppa, Katrin; Mani, Nivedita; Peperkamp, Sharon – Child Development, 2013
Using a picture pointing task, this study examines toddlers' processing of phonological alternations that trigger sound changes in connected speech. Three experiments investigate whether 2;5- to 3-year-old children take into account assimilations--processes by which phonological features of one sound spread to adjacent sounds--for the purpose of…
Descriptors: Task Analysis, Toddlers, Language Processing, Phonology
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Robertson, Erin K.; Shi, Rushen; Melancon, Andreane – Child Development, 2012
Function words support many aspects of language acquisition. This study investigated whether toddlers understand the number feature of determiners and use it for noun comprehension. French offers an ideal "test case" as number is phonetically marked in determiners but not in nouns. Twenty French-learning 24-month-olds completed a split-screen…
Descriptors: Language Acquisition, French, Toddlers, Nouns
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Weller, Drika; Lagattuta, Kristin Hansen – Child Development, 2013
Five- to 13-year-old European American children ("N" = 76) predicted characters' decisions, emotions, and obligations in prosocial moral dilemmas. Across age, children judged that characters would feel more positive emotions helping an unfamiliar child from the racial in-group versus out-group (African American), happier ignoring the…
Descriptors: Emotional Response, Whites, Prosocial Behavior, Moral Values
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Quinn, Paul C.; Intraub, Helene – Child Development, 2007
This investigation examined whether infants display "boundary extension"--a tendency to remember more of a visual scene than was presented. Three- to 7-month-olds were familiarized with a photograph of a visual scene, and tested with wide-angle versus close-up views of the scene. Infants preferred the close-up, indicating that they perceived the…
Descriptors: Photography, Infants, Pictorial Stimuli, Visual Perception
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Wellman, Henry M. – Child Development, 1977
Kindergarten, first-, and third-grade children were presented depicted items and asked to name them. For each item they could not name they were asked to judge (1) if they felt they knew the name and so would be able to recognize it and (2) if they had seen the depicted item before. (Author/JMB)
Descriptors: Memory, Pictorial Stimuli, Primary Education, Recognition
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Carr, Thomas H.; And Others – Child Development, 1977
The effect of three different kinds of advance descriptions on recognition memory for component information from pictures was measured for 72 first-grade children. All descriptions resulted in higher retention of all components than viewing without description. (Author/JMB)
Descriptors: Elementary Education, Memory, Pictorial Stimuli, Recognition
Previous Page | Next Page »
Pages: 1  |  2  |  3  |  4  |  5  |  6  |  7