Publication Date
In 2025 | 1 |
Since 2024 | 4 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 8 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 18 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 113 |
Descriptor
Primatology | 197 |
Animals | 49 |
Animal Behavior | 38 |
Evolution | 34 |
Infants | 28 |
Cognitive Processes | 27 |
Comparative Analysis | 19 |
Anthropology | 16 |
Models | 16 |
Child Development | 15 |
Foreign Countries | 15 |
More ▼ |
Source
Author
Call, Josep | 8 |
Tomasello, Michael | 8 |
Santos, Laurie R. | 5 |
Brannon, Elizabeth M. | 4 |
Ghazanfar, Asif A. | 3 |
Hauser, Marc D. | 3 |
Lyn, Heidi | 3 |
Matsuzawa, Tetsuro | 3 |
Suomi, Stephen J. | 3 |
Akuffo, Patrick B. | 2 |
Barnes, Jennifer L. | 2 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Education Level
Higher Education | 5 |
Elementary Education | 3 |
Early Childhood Education | 2 |
Middle Schools | 2 |
Secondary Education | 2 |
Adult Education | 1 |
Grade 3 | 1 |
Grade 4 | 1 |
Grade 5 | 1 |
Grade 6 | 1 |
High Schools | 1 |
More ▼ |
Location
Germany | 2 |
Japan | 2 |
Spain | 2 |
Africa | 1 |
Argentina | 1 |
Brazil | 1 |
California | 1 |
Congo | 1 |
Congo Republic | 1 |
Europe | 1 |
Finland | 1 |
More ▼ |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Education for All Handicapped… | 1 |
Individuals with Disabilities… | 1 |
Individuals with Disabilities… | 1 |
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Vouloumanos, Athena; Hauser, Marc D.; Werker, Janet F.; Martin, Alia – Child Development, 2010
Human neonates prefer listening to speech compared to many nonspeech sounds, suggesting that humans are born with a bias for speech. However, neonates' preference may derive from properties of speech that are not unique but instead are shared with the vocalizations of other species. To test this, thirty neonates and sixteen 3-month-olds were…
Descriptors: Neonates, Primatology, Auditory Stimuli, Speech Communication
Povinelli, Daniel J.; Reaux, James E.; Frey, Scott H. – Neuropsychologia, 2010
Considerable attention has been devoted to behaviors in which tools are used to perform actions in extrapersonal space by extending the reach. Evidence suggests that these behaviors result in an expansion of the body schema and peripersonal space. However, humans often use tools to perform tasks within peripersonal space that cannot be…
Descriptors: Evolution, Primatology, Psychomotor Skills, Schemata (Cognition)
Corballis, Michael C. – Brain and Language, 2010
The mirror system provided a natural platform for the subsequent evolution of language. In nonhuman primates, the system provides for the understanding of biological action, and possibly for imitation, both prerequisites for language. I argue that language evolved from manual gestures, initially as a system of pantomime, but with gestures…
Descriptors: Nonverbal Communication, Brain Hemisphere Functions, Primatology, Evolution
Zhang, Jiabei – Physical Educator, 2011
The purpose of this study was to analyze quantitative needs for more adapted physical education (APE) teachers based on both market- and prevalence-based models. The market-based need for more APE teachers was examined based on APE teacher positions funded, while the prevalence-based need for additional APE teachers was analyzed based on students…
Descriptors: Adapted Physical Education, Incidence, Primatology, Physical Education Teachers
Buttelmann, David; Call, Josep; Tomasello, Michael – Developmental Science, 2009
Although apes understand others' goals and perceptions, little is known about their understanding of others' emotional expressions. We conducted three studies following the general paradigm of Repacholi and colleagues (1997, 1998). In Study 1, a human reacted emotionally to the hidden contents of two boxes, after which the ape was allowed to…
Descriptors: Nonverbal Communication, Primatology, Animals, Emotional Response
Cacchione, Trix; Call, Josep – Cognition, 2010
Recent research suggests that witnessing events of fission (e.g., the splitting of a solid object) impairs human infants', human adults', and non-human primates' object representations. The present studies investigated the reactions of gorillas and orangutans to cohesion violation across different types of fission events implementing a behavioral…
Descriptors: Object Permanence, Infants, Primatology, Cognitive Development
Bullock, Christopher E.; Myers, Todd M. – Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 2009
Acquisition and maintenance of touch-screen responding was examined in naive cynomolgus monkeys ("Macaca fascicularis") under automaintenance and classical conditioning arrangements. In the first condition of Experiment 1, we compared acquisition of screen touching to a randomly positioned stimulus (a gray square) that was either stationary or…
Descriptors: Primatology, Classical Conditioning, Stimuli, Tactual Perception
Seegmiller, Janelle K.; Watson, Jason M.; Strayer, David L. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2011
Inattentional blindness refers to the finding that people do not always see what appears in their gaze. Though inattentional blindness affects large percentages of people, it is unclear if there are individual differences in susceptibility. The present study addressed whether individual differences in attentional control, as reflected by…
Descriptors: Video Technology, Blindness, Primatology, Short Term Memory
Merritt, Dustin J.; Casasanto, Daniel; Brannon, Elizabeth M. – Cognition, 2010
Research on the relationship between the representation of space and time has produced two contrasting proposals. ATOM posits that space and time are represented via a common magnitude system, suggesting a symmetrical relationship between space and time. According to metaphor theory, however, representations of time depend on representations of…
Descriptors: Nonverbal Communication, Figurative Language, Primatology, Animals
Taubert, Jessica; Parr, Lisa A. – Brain and Cognition, 2009
Humans are subject to the composite illusion: two identical top halves of a face are perceived as "different" when they are presented with different bottom halves. This observation suggests that when building a mental representation of a face, the underlying system perceives the whole face, and has difficulty decomposing facial features. We…
Descriptors: Primatology, Visual Perception, Human Body, Cognitive Processes
Phillips, Webb; Barnes, Jennifer L.; Mahajan, Neha; Yamaguchi, Mariko; Santos, Laurie R. – Developmental Science, 2009
A sensitivity to the intentions behind human action is a crucial developmental achievement in infants. Is this intention reading ability a unique and relatively recent product of human evolution and culture, or does this capacity instead have roots in our non-human primate ancestors? Recent work by Call and colleagues (2004) lends credence to the…
Descriptors: Evolution, Intention, Primatology, Animals
Merritt, Dustin J.; Rugani, Rosa; Brannon, Elizabeth M. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 2009
The goal of the current research was to explore whether monkeys possess conceptual precursors necessary for understanding zero. We trained rhesus monkeys on a nonsymbolic numerical matching-to-sample task, and on a numerical ordering task. We then introduced nondifferentially reinforced trials that contained empty sets to determine whether monkeys…
Descriptors: Primatology, Mathematical Concepts, Symbols (Mathematics), Objective Tests
Browning, N. Andrew; Grossberg, Stephen; Mingolla, Ennio – Cognitive Psychology, 2009
Visually-based navigation is a key competence during spatial cognition. Animals avoid obstacles and approach goals in novel cluttered environments using optic flow to compute heading with respect to the environment. Most navigation models try either explain data, or to demonstrate navigational competence in real-world environments without regard…
Descriptors: Optics, Visual Perception, Spatial Ability, Schemata (Cognition)
Press, Clare; Bird, Geoffrey; Walsh, Eamonn; Heyes, Cecilia – Brain and Cognition, 2008
Previous research has indicated a potential discontinuity between monkey and human ventral premotor-parietal mirror systems, namely that monkey mirror systems process only transitive (object-directed) actions, whereas human mirror systems may also process intransitive (non-object-directed) actions. The present study investigated this discontinuity…
Descriptors: Reaction Time, Imitation, Primatology, Responses
Apperly, Ian A.; Carroll, Daniel J. – Developmental Science, 2009
In two experiments, 330 3- to 4-year-olds competed for stickers in a game in which the optimal response strategy was to point to an empty box that their opponent would receive in order to obtain a baited box for themselves. When the baited box contained stickers, children showed a strong tendency to point at the baited box and therefore lose the…
Descriptors: Photography, Food, Responses, Cognitive Processes