Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 2 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 12 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 47 |
Descriptor
Probability | 48 |
Vocabulary Development | 48 |
Language Acquisition | 17 |
Mathematics Instruction | 11 |
Phonology | 11 |
Second Language Instruction | 11 |
Algebra | 10 |
Computer Literacy | 10 |
Cooperative Learning | 10 |
Creativity | 10 |
Data Analysis | 10 |
More ▼ |
Source
Author
Storkel, Holly L. | 4 |
Evans, Julia L. | 2 |
Groth, Randall E. | 2 |
Maekawa, Junko | 2 |
Abdon, Buenafe, Comp. | 1 |
Alt, Mary | 1 |
Aschenbrenner, Andrew J. | 1 |
Atabas, Sebnem | 1 |
Austin, Jathan W. | 1 |
Bai, Xuejun | 1 |
Baker, Anne | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 36 |
Reports - Research | 31 |
Reports - Descriptive | 11 |
Reports - Evaluative | 3 |
Collected Works - Proceedings | 1 |
Dissertations/Theses -… | 1 |
Numerical/Quantitative Data | 1 |
Reference Materials -… | 1 |
Education Level
Elementary Education | 8 |
Preschool Education | 5 |
Early Childhood Education | 4 |
Elementary Secondary Education | 4 |
Grade 1 | 4 |
Grade 2 | 4 |
Grade 4 | 4 |
Grade 5 | 4 |
Grade 6 | 4 |
Grade 7 | 4 |
Grade 8 | 4 |
More ▼ |
Audience
Location
Arizona | 10 |
Germany | 2 |
Netherlands | 2 |
Philippines | 2 |
United States | 2 |
Asia | 1 |
Australia | 1 |
Belgium | 1 |
Brazil | 1 |
Burma | 1 |
Cambodia | 1 |
More ▼ |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
No Child Left Behind Act 2001 | 9 |
Assessments and Surveys
Peabody Picture Vocabulary… | 2 |
Expressive One Word Picture… | 1 |
Goldman Fristoe Test of… | 1 |
MacArthur Communicative… | 1 |
Mean Length of Utterance | 1 |
Oral and Written Language… | 1 |
Wechsler Preschool and… | 1 |
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Jones, Gary; Cabiddu, Francesco; Barrett, Doug J. K.; Castro, Antonio; Lee, Bethany – First Language, 2023
Child-directed speech has long been known to influence children's vocabulary learning. However, while we know that caregiver utterances differ from those directed at adults in various ways, little is known about any differences in the lexical properties of child-directed and adult-directed utterances. We compare over half a million word tokens…
Descriptors: Child Language, Vocabulary Development, Caregiver Child Relationship, Phonemes
Findley, Kelly; Whitacre, Ian; Atabas, Sebnem – Teaching Statistics: An International Journal for Teachers, 2021
Providing opportunities for students to express ideas in their own words is key to promoting authentic disciplinary engagement and robust conceptual understanding. In this article, we discuss our experiences and observations related to students' informal vocabulary in the context of a middle school probability unit. We noticed that students'…
Descriptors: Naming, Vocabulary Development, Informal Education, Probability
Groth, Randall E.; Bergner, Jennifer A.; Austin, Jathan W. – Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 2020
Normative discourse about probability requires shared meanings for disciplinary vocabulary. Previous research indicates that students' meanings for probability vocabulary often differ from those of mathematicians, creating a need to attend to developing students' use of language. Current standards documents conflict in their recommendations about…
Descriptors: Mathematics Education, Mathematics Instruction, Probability, Vocabulary Development
Stevens, Jon Scott; Gleitman, Lila R.; Trueswell, John C.; Yang, Charles – Cognitive Science, 2017
We evaluate here the performance of four models of cross-situational word learning: two global models, which extract and retain multiple referential alternatives from each word occurrence; and two local models, which extract just a single referent from each occurrence. One of these local models, dubbed "Pursuit," uses an associative…
Descriptors: Semantics, Associative Learning, Probability, Computational Linguistics
Kühhirt, Michael; Klein, Markus – Child Development, 2018
This study investigates the relationship between early maternal employment history and children's vocabulary and inductive reasoning ability at age 5, drawing on longitudinal information on 2,200 children from the Growing Up in Scotland data. Prior research rarely addresses dynamics in maternal employment and the methodological ramifications of…
Descriptors: Mothers, Employed Parents, Child Development, Correlation
Bunce, John P.; Scott, Rose M. – Journal of Child Language, 2017
While recent studies suggest children can use cross-situational information to learn words, these studies involved minimal referential ambiguity, and the cross-situational evidence overwhelmingly favored a single referent for each word. Here we asked whether 2-5-year-olds could identify a noun's referent when the scene and cross-situational…
Descriptors: Ambiguity (Semantics), Toddlers, Young Children, Evidence
Groth, Randall E.; Butler, Jaime; Nelson, Delmar – Teaching Statistics: An International Journal for Teachers, 2016
Students can struggle to understand and use terms that describe probabilities. Such struggles lead to difficulties comprehending classroom conversations. In this article, we describe some specific misunderstandings a group of students (ages 11-12) held in regard to vocabulary such as "certain", "likely" and…
Descriptors: Statistical Analysis, Statistics, Probability, Misconceptions
Bordag, Denisa; Kirschenbaum, Amit; Rogahn, Maria; Tschirner, Erwin – Second Language Research, 2017
Four experiments were conducted to examine the role of orthotactic probability, i.e. the sequential letter probability, in the early stages of vocabulary acquisition by adult native speakers and advanced learners of German. The results show different effects for orthographic probability in incidental and intentional vocabulary acquisition: Whereas…
Descriptors: Role, Probability, Vocabulary Development, Advanced Students
Verhagen, Josje; de Bree, Elise; Mulder, Hanna; Leseman, Paul – Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 2017
This study investigates the relationship between nonword repetition (NWR) and vocabulary in 2-year-olds. Questions addressed are whether (1) NWR and vocabulary are associated, (2) phonotactic probability affects NWR, and (3) there is an interaction effect between phonotactic probability and vocabulary on NWR performance. The general aim of the…
Descriptors: Correlation, Toddlers, Vocabulary Development, English
Bartolotti, James; Marian, Viorica – Language Learning, 2017
Learning a new language involves substantial vocabulary acquisition. Learners can accelerate this process by relying on words with native-language overlap, such as cognates. For bilingual third language learners, it is necessary to determine how their two existing languages interact during novel language learning. A scaffolding account predicts…
Descriptors: Bilingual Students, Bilingual Education, Adults, Vocabulary Development
Testolin, Alberto; Stoianov, Ivilin; Sperduti, Alessandro; Zorzi, Marco – Cognitive Science, 2016
Learning the structure of event sequences is a ubiquitous problem in cognition and particularly in language. One possible solution is to learn a probabilistic generative model of sequences that allows making predictions about upcoming events. Though appealing from a neurobiological standpoint, this approach is typically not pursued in…
Descriptors: Orthographic Symbols, Neurological Organization, Models, Probability
McKean, Cristina; Letts, Carolyn; Howard, David – Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 2013
Purpose: In this study, the authors aimed to explore the relationship between lexical and phonological knowledge in children with primary language impairment (PLI) through the application of a developmental methodology. Specifically, they tested whether there is evidence for an impairment in the process of phonological abstraction in this group of…
Descriptors: Children, Language Impairments, Phonology, Probability
Honoré, Nastasya; Noël, Marie-Pascale – Journal of Education and Training Studies, 2017
Working memory capacities are associated with mathematical development. Many studies have tried to improve working memory abilities through training. Furthermore, the central executive has been shown to be the component of working memory, which is the most strongly related to numerical and arithmetical skills. Therefore, we developed a training…
Descriptors: Short Term Memory, Kindergarten, Randomized Controlled Trials, Training
Rispens, Judith; Baker, Anne; Duinmeijer, Iris – Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 2015
Purpose: The effects of neighborhood density (ND) and lexical frequency on word recognition and the effects of phonotactic probability (PP) on nonword repetition (NWR) were examined to gain insight into processing at the lexical and sublexical levels in typically developing (TD) children and children with developmental language problems. Method:…
Descriptors: Word Recognition, Probability, Repetition, Language Processing
Storkel, Holly L.; Bontempo, Daniel E.; Aschenbrenner, Andrew J.; Maekawa, Junko; Lee, Su-Yeon – Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 2013
Purpose: Phonotactic probability or neighborhood density has predominately been defined through the use of gross distinctions (i.e., low vs. high). In the current studies, the authors examined the influence of finer changes in probability (Experiment 1) and density (Experiment 2) on word learning.
Method: The authors examined the full range of…
Descriptors: Probability, Vocabulary Development, Novelty (Stimulus Dimension), Pictorial Stimuli