Publication Date
In 2025 | 1 |
Since 2024 | 1 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 1 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 1 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 2 |
Descriptor
Problem Solving | 6 |
Children | 4 |
Conflict Resolution | 4 |
Conflict | 2 |
Mothers | 2 |
Sibling Relationship | 2 |
Age Differences | 1 |
Attitude Change | 1 |
Behavior Problems | 1 |
Child Development | 1 |
Childrens Attitudes | 1 |
More ▼ |
Source
Child Development | 6 |
Author
Brody, Gene H. | 1 |
Cicchetti, Dante | 1 |
Davies, Patrick T. | 1 |
Elfriede R. Holstein | 1 |
Enright, Robert D. | 1 |
Garvin Brod | 1 |
Judd, Susan A. | 1 |
Leonie S. Weindorf | 1 |
Manning, Liviah G. | 1 |
Maria Theobald | 1 |
Mervis, Carolyn B. | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 6 |
Reports - Research | 6 |
Education Level
Audience
Location
Germany | 1 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Elfriede R. Holstein; Maria Theobald; Leonie S. Weindorf; Garvin Brod – Child Development, 2025
We investigated the role of children's conflict monitoring skills in revising an intuitive scientific theory. Children aged 5 to 9 (N = 177; 53% girls, data collected in Germany from 2019-2023) completed computer-based tasks on water displacement, a concept prone to misconceptions. Children predicted which of two objects would displace more water…
Descriptors: Children, Conflict, Task Analysis, Foreign Countries
Davies, Patrick T.; Manning, Liviah G.; Cicchetti, Dante – Child Development, 2013
This study examined whether children’s difficulties with stage-salient tasks served as an explanatory mechanism in the pathway between their insecurity in the interparental relationship and their disruptive behavior problems. Using a multimethod, multi-informant design, 201 two-year-old children and their mothers participated in 3 annual…
Descriptors: Security (Psychology), Parent Child Relationship, Behavior Problems, Structural Equation Models

Ram, Avigail; Ross, Hildy S. – Child Development, 2001
Observed in a laboratory setting how dyads, ages 4 and 6 years or 6 and 8 years, negotiated division of toys. Found that children used a preponderance of constructive problem-solving strategies rather than contentious tactics. Degree of conflict of interests and quality of sibling relationships predicted use of problem-solving and contentious…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Children, Conflict Resolution, Predictor Variables

Judd, Susan A.; Mervis, Carolyn B. – Child Development, 1979
The results of two experiments showed that five year olds can learn to solve class-inclusion problems if they are forced to consider the contradiction between their incorrect answers and their correct counting of the superordinate and subordinate classes. (JMB)
Descriptors: Classification, Computation, Conflict Resolution, Early Childhood Education

Enright, Robert D.; Sutterfield, Sara J. – Child Development, 1980
Two classrooms of first graders (N=40) were administered Damon's moral judgment measure, Shure and Spivack's social problem solving measure, and the Stanford-Binet vocabulary. Concurrently, two observers in the children's school environment recorded incidences of successful resolutions of interactions, amount of derogation, and the number of times…
Descriptors: Cognitive Development, Conflict Resolution, Elementary School Students, Moral Development

Brody, Gene H.; And Others – Child Development, 1992
Examined the relationship between sibling conflict and marital satisfaction, spousal conflict, family emotional climate, and family problem-solving processes that involve siblings. Results indicated that equal treatment by parents, family harmony in family discussions of sibling problems, and parent perceptions of family cohesiveness were…
Descriptors: Children, Conflict Resolution, Family Communication, Family Environment