Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 0 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 5 |
Descriptor
Cognitive Processes | 6 |
Problem Solving | 6 |
College Students | 3 |
Experimental Psychology | 2 |
Nonverbal Communication | 2 |
Priming | 2 |
Spatial Ability | 2 |
Abstract Reasoning | 1 |
Addition | 1 |
Arithmetic | 1 |
Attention | 1 |
More ▼ |
Source
Journal of Experimental… | 6 |
Author
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 6 |
Reports - Evaluative | 3 |
Reports - Research | 3 |
Speeches/Meeting Papers | 1 |
Education Level
Higher Education | 3 |
Postsecondary Education | 2 |
Audience
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Ric, Francois; Muller, Dominique – Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 2012
This research shows that people can unconsciously initiate and follow arithmetic rules (e.g., addition). Participants were asked to detect whether a symbol was a digit. This symbol was preceded by 2 digits and a subliminal instruction: "add" or a control instruction. Participants were faster at identifying a symbol as a number when the…
Descriptors: Arithmetic, Cognitive Processes, Problem Solving, Numbers
Hills, Thomas T.; Todd, Peter M.; Goldstone, Robert L. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 2010
The trade-off between exploration and exploitation is common to a wide variety of problems involving search in space and mind. The prevalence of this trade-off and its neurological underpinnings led us to propose domain-general cognitive search processes (Hills, Todd, & Goldstone, 2008). We propose further that these are consistent with the…
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Priming, Problem Solving, Goal Orientation
Patsenko, Elena G.; Altmann, Erik M. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 2010
Routine human behavior has often been attributed to plans--mental representations of sequences goals and actions--but can also be attributed to more opportunistic interactions of mind and a structured environment. This study asks whether performance on a task traditionally analyzed in terms of plans can be better understood from a "situated" (or…
Descriptors: Eye Movements, Attention, Experimental Psychology, Memory
Chu, Mingyuan; Kita, Sotaro – Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 2008
This study investigated the motor strategy involved in mental rotation tasks by examining 2 types of spontaneous gestures (hand-object interaction gestures, representing the agentive hand action on an object, vs. object-movement gestures, representing the movement of an object by itself) and different types of verbal descriptions of rotation.…
Descriptors: Interaction, Spatial Ability, Nonverbal Communication, Cognitive Processes
Broaders, Sara C.; Cook, Susan Wagner; Mitchell, Zachary; Goldin-Meadow, Susan – Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 2007
Speakers routinely gesture with their hands when they talk, and those gestures often convey information not found anywhere in their speech. This information is typically not consciously accessible, yet it provides an early sign that the speaker is ready to learn a particular task (S. Goldin-Meadow, 2003). In this sense, the unwitting gestures that…
Descriptors: Learning Readiness, Mathematics Instruction, Cognitive Processes, Nonverbal Communication

Sternberg, Robert J. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 1980
Four experiments compared three alternative models of linear syllogistic reasoning: (1) linguistic; (2) spatial; and (3) mixed linguistic-spatial. The mixed model, indicating the importance of both verbal and spatial ability, was supported by all four experiments, and for about three-fourths of the undergraduate students studied. (Author/GDC)
Descriptors: Abstract Reasoning, Cognitive Processes, Cues, Higher Education