Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 1 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 4 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 7 |
Descriptor
Cognitive Development | 16 |
Difficulty Level | 16 |
Problem Solving | 16 |
Cognitive Processes | 9 |
Age Differences | 6 |
Elementary School Students | 4 |
Teaching Methods | 4 |
Foreign Countries | 3 |
Student Behavior | 3 |
Thinking Skills | 3 |
Attention | 2 |
More ▼ |
Source
Author
Acredolo, Curt | 1 |
Alibali, Martha W. | 1 |
Barratt, Barnaby B. | 1 |
Bjorklund, David F. | 1 |
Brown, Sarah A. | 1 |
Brown, Seth | 1 |
Chick, Helen L., Ed. | 1 |
Decker, Scott L. | 1 |
Ephraim Zulu | 1 |
Feller, Thomas, Jr. | 1 |
Gardiner, Amy K. | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 9 |
Reports - Research | 8 |
Reports - Descriptive | 2 |
Reports - Evaluative | 2 |
Collected Works - Proceedings | 1 |
Tests/Questionnaires | 1 |
Education Level
Elementary Education | 3 |
Higher Education | 3 |
Secondary Education | 3 |
Elementary Secondary Education | 2 |
Middle Schools | 2 |
Postsecondary Education | 2 |
Grade 5 | 1 |
Intermediate Grades | 1 |
Junior High Schools | 1 |
Audience
Teachers | 2 |
Researchers | 1 |
Location
South Africa | 2 |
Australia | 1 |
Indonesia | 1 |
Iran | 1 |
Israel | 1 |
Japan | 1 |
North Carolina | 1 |
Singapore | 1 |
South Korea | 1 |
Sri Lanka | 1 |
Taiwan | 1 |
More ▼ |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
Program for International… | 1 |
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Brown, Seth; Feller, Thomas, Jr. – Educational Leadership, 2022
Self-directed student learning begins with self-directed educators. Teachers in Greenville, North Carolina, are training in the Habits of Mind, a series of thinking behaviors that support self-directed learning by helping students successfully tackle complex problems. The authors outline key instructional strategies that have grown from these…
Descriptors: Independent Study, Self Management, Student Behavior, Teacher Education
Hallman-Thrasher, Allyson; Spangler, Denise A. – Mathematics Teacher: Learning and Teaching PK-12, 2020
We share ideas for preparing for and enacting high-cognitive demand tasks in ways that support students in articulating and justifying their ideas. We offer strategies for developing and posing several types of purposeful questions: (1) eliciting thinking, (2) generating ideas, (3) clarifying explanations, and (4) justifying claims.
Descriptors: Teaching Methods, Questioning Techniques, Thinking Skills, Cognitive Development
Alibali, Martha W.; Brown, Sarah A.; Menendez, David – Grantee Submission, 2019
Learning, development, and response to instruction often involve changes in the strategies that learners use to solve problems. In this chapter, our focus is on mathematical problem solving in both children and adults. We offer a selective review of research on three classes of factors that may influence processes of strategy change in…
Descriptors: Learning Strategies, Problem Solving, Mathematics Skills, Mathematics Instruction
Ephraim Zulu; Theodore Haupt; Vittorio Tramontin – Problems of Education in the 21st Century, 2018
The aim of the research was to explore the levels of cognitive loading induced by certain tenets of constructivist pedagogy namely self-directed learning complex questions and zone of proximal development. The study also sought to investigate the associations between these constructs and with cognitive loading and complex questions. Data for the…
Descriptors: Foreign Countries, Construction Management, College Students, Independent Study
Decker, Scott L.; Roberts, Alycia M. – Psychology in the Schools, 2015
Development of early math skill depends on a prerequisite level of cognitive development. Identification of specific cognitive skills that are important for math development may not only inform instructional approaches but also inform assessment approaches to identifying children with specific learning problems in math. This study investigated the…
Descriptors: Elementary School Mathematics, Mathematics Skills, Problem Solving, Cognitive Development
Gardiner, Amy K.; Bjorklund, David F.; Greif, Marissa L.; Gray, Sarah K. – Cognitive Development, 2012
Children's acquisition of tool use abilities is an important part of development but is not yet well understood. This study compares two modes of tool-use learning, observation and individual haptic experience. Two- and 3-year-olds had haptic experience with tools, observed tool use by others, had both haptic and observational experience, or no…
Descriptors: Observation, Task Analysis, Difficulty Level, Cognitive Ability
Liu, Min; Horton, Lucas; Toprac, Paul; Yuen, Timothy T. – Educational Media and Technology Yearbook, 2012
This chapter examines the various cognitive tools embedded in a multimedia-enriched PBL environment for middle school science known as "Alien Rescue" and shares the research findings of the use of these cognitive tools in assisting young learners' problem solving. The goal of this chapter is to illustrate strategies for designing…
Descriptors: Middle School Students, Science Instruction, Problem Based Learning, Teaching Methods

Acredolo, Curt; Horobin, Karen – Developmental Psychology, 1977
First-, third-, fifth-, and sixth-grade children were administered 20 relational reasoning problems in which they had to deduce the possible sizes of one item relative to two others on the basis of a visual comparison and a written clue. Dramatic differences were observed between fifth- and sixth-grade children. Corrective feedback improved…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Cognitive Development, Difficulty Level, Elementary Education

Goodwin, Kathryn S.; Turner, Ralph R. – International Journal of Behavioral Development, 1981
Examined effects of cognitive focusing training in early and late concrete operational children. Focusing was manifested by the late concrete operational children regardless of whether or not they had been trained. The amount of negative feedback and the nature of the probe techniques affected the manifestation of focusing. (Author/DB)
Descriptors: Children, Cognitive Development, Cognitive Processes, Difficulty Level

Barratt, Barnaby B. – Developmental Psychology, 1975
This study investigated the emergence of combinatorial competence in early adolescence and the effectiveness of a programmed discovery training procedure. Significant increases in combinatorial skill with age were shown; it was found that the expression of this skill was significantly facilitated if problems involved concrete material of low…
Descriptors: Abstract Reasoning, Adolescents, Cognitive Development, Difficulty Level

Ronning, Royce R. – American Educational Research Journal, 1977
Evidence is provided for gradual age (and, seemingly, cognitive developmental) changes in the acquisition of complex problem solving strategies. Differential performance as a function of exposure to a child-model exhibiting the best strategy also suggests the role of learning in strategy acquisition. (Author/MV)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Behavior Change, Cognitive Development, Cognitive Processes
White, Sheldon H. – 1973
This final report discusses research and literature analyses concerned with the attentional mechanisms in children's learning and analysis of cognitive changes from ages 5 to 7. The long-term objective of the project was to relate knowledge about children's attention and learning to issues of educational practice. The project focused its efforts…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Attention, Attention Span, Cognitive Development

Murphy, Catherine M.; Wood, David J. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1981
Using information contained in nine photographs, young children constructed a wooden pyramid. Control children were given the same task but no pictorial information. Children's performances were significantly better when pictorial information was available. Sex and age differences were noted regarding strategies used to complete the task.…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Attention, Cognitive Development, Difficulty Level

Steinberg, Esther R. – Contemporary Educational Psychology, 1980
Faced with a problem in which the probability of obtaining the correct answer was 70 percent, 2 of 23 kindergartners and 18 of 19 second graders generated an appropriate strategy. When the probability of a chance correct response was reduced to .45, 23 of 35 kindergartners generated an appropriate strategy. (Author/GDC)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Cognitive Development, Cognitive Processes, Computer Assisted Instruction
Uzgiris, Ina C. – 1977
This paper describes seven interrelated studies concerned with children's understanding of sequential actions and with the effects of observing a model on this understanding. A total of 546 elementary and secondary school students served as subjects for the studies. The tasks for all of the studies involved deriving the pattern for a sequence from…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Cognitive Development, Cognitive Processes, Difficulty Level
Previous Page | Next Page ยป
Pages: 1 | 2