Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 4 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 21 |
Descriptor
Color | 21 |
Experimental Psychology | 21 |
Reaction Time | 21 |
Visual Stimuli | 15 |
Attention | 8 |
Cognitive Processes | 8 |
Foreign Countries | 7 |
Undergraduate Students | 7 |
Visual Perception | 7 |
Cues | 6 |
Responses | 6 |
More ▼ |
Source
Journal of Experimental… | 13 |
Journal of Experimental… | 4 |
Journal of Experimental… | 2 |
Psicologica: International… | 2 |
Author
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 21 |
Reports - Research | 18 |
Reports - Evaluative | 2 |
Opinion Papers | 1 |
Education Level
Higher Education | 10 |
Postsecondary Education | 7 |
Audience
Location
Netherlands | 2 |
Australia | 1 |
Illinois | 1 |
Missouri | 1 |
North Carolina | 1 |
Spain | 1 |
Switzerland | 1 |
United Kingdom (England) | 1 |
United Kingdom (Scotland) | 1 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
Stroop Color Word Test | 6 |
Wechsler Adult Intelligence… | 1 |
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Cohen-Shikora, Emily R.; Suh, Jihyun; Bugg, Julie M. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2019
In this article, we assess an alternative account of a key experimental pattern thought to index top-down control. The list-wide proportion congruence effect is the well-documented pattern whereby the congruency effect (i.e., Stroop effect) is attenuated in lists containing mostly incongruent trials relative to lists containing mostly congruent…
Descriptors: Congruence (Psychology), Reaction Time, Color, Conflict
Kinoshita, Sachiko; De Wit, Bianca; Norris, Dennis – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2017
In 2 variants of the color-word Stroop task, we compared 5 types of color-neutral distractors--real words (e.g., "HAT"), pseudowords (e.g., "HIX"), consonant strings (e.g., "HDK"), symbol strings (e.g., #$%), and a row of Xs (e.g., "XXX")--as well as incongruent color words (e.g., "GREEN" displayed…
Descriptors: Color, Interference (Learning), Reaction Time, Visual Stimuli
Gregory, Samantha E. A.; Jackson, Margaret C. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2017
Joint attention--the mutual focus of 2 individuals on an item--speeds detection and discrimination of target information. However, what happens to that information beyond the initial perceptual episode? To fully comprehend and engage with our immediate environment also requires working memory (WM), which integrates information from second to…
Descriptors: Attention, Short Term Memory, Eye Movements, Cues
Shao, Zeshu; Roelofs, Ardi; Martin, Randi C.; Meyer, Antje S. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2015
In 2 studies, we examined whether explicit distractors are necessary and sufficient to evoke selective inhibition in 3 naming tasks: the semantic blocking, picture-word interference, and color-word Stroop task. Delta plots were used to quantify the size of the interference effects as a function of reaction time (RT). Selective inhibition was…
Descriptors: Inhibition, Pictorial Stimuli, Semantics, Interference (Learning)
Wantz, Andrea L.; Borst, Grégoire; Mast, Fred W.; Lobmaier, Janek S. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2015
Mental color imagery abilities are commonly measured using paradigms that involve naming, judging, or comparing the colors of visual mental images of well-known objects (e.g., "Is a sunflower darker yellow than a lemon"?). Although this approach is widely used in patient studies, differences in the ability to perform such color…
Descriptors: Experimental Psychology, Color, Imagery, Visual Stimuli
Harrison, Neil R.; McCann, Amy – Psicologica: International Journal of Methodology and Experimental Psychology, 2014
Attentional bias plays an important role in the development and maintenance of alcohol addiction, and has often been measured with a visual probe task, where reaction times are compared for probes replacing either a substance-related cue or a neutral cue. Systematic low-level differences between image classes are a potential cause of low internal…
Descriptors: Color, Attention, Addictive Behavior, Alcohol Abuse
Segaert, Katrien; Weber, Kirsten; Cladder-Micus, Mira; Hagoort, Peter – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2014
Speakers sometimes repeat syntactic structures across sentences, a phenomenon called syntactic priming. We investigated the influence of verb-bound syntactic preferences on syntactic priming effects in response choices and response latencies for German ditransitive sentences. In the response choices we found "inverse preference effects":…
Descriptors: Experimental Psychology, Verbs, Syntax, Priming
Bissett, Patrick G.; Logan, Gordon D. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2012
Performance in the stop-signal paradigm involves a balance between going and stopping, and one way that this balance is struck is through shifting priority away from the go task, slowing responses after a stop signal, and improving the probability of inhibition. In 6 experiments, the authors tested whether there is a corresponding shift in…
Descriptors: Inhibition, Probability, Reaction Time, Experimental Psychology
Jones, Manon W.; Snowling, Margaret J.; Moll, Kristina – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2016
Reading fluency is often predicted by rapid automatized naming (RAN) speed, which as the name implies, measures the automaticity with which familiar stimuli (e.g., letters) can be retrieved and named. Readers with dyslexia are considered to have less "automatized" access to lexical information, reflected in longer RAN times compared with…
Descriptors: Reading Fluency, Dyslexia, Interference (Learning), Color
Gul, Amara; Humphreys, Glyn W. – Psicologica: International Journal of Methodology and Experimental Psychology, 2015
Congruency effects were examined using a manual response version of the Stroop task in which the relationship between the colour word and its hue on incongruent trials was either kept constant or varied randomly across different pairings within the stimulus set. Congruency effects were increased in the condition where the incongruent hue-word…
Descriptors: Experiments, Psychological Testing, Perceptual Development, Perception Tests
Roelofs, Ardi – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2014
Investigators have found no agreement on the functional locus of Stroop interference in vocal naming. Whereas it has long been assumed that the interference arises during spoken word planning, more recently some investigators have revived an account from the 1960s and 1970s holding that the interference occurs in an articulatory buffer after word…
Descriptors: Eye Movements, Interference (Language), Naming, Pictorial Stimuli
McVay, Jennifer C.; Kane, Michael J. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2012
A combined experimental, individual-differences, and thought-sampling study tested the predictions of executive attention (e.g., Engle & Kane, 2004) and coordinative binding (e.g., Oberauer, Suss, Wilhelm, & Sander, 2007) theories of working memory capacity (WMC). We assessed 288 subjects' WMC and their performance and mind-wandering rates…
Descriptors: Performance Factors, Reaction Time, Short Term Memory, Predictor Variables
Parmentier, Fabrice B. R.; Hebrero, Maria – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2013
It is well established that a task-irrelevant sound (deviant sound) departing from an otherwise repetitive sequence of sounds (standard sounds) elicits an involuntary capture of attention and orienting response toward the deviant stimulus, resulting in the lengthening of response times in an ongoing task. Some have argued that this type of…
Descriptors: Acoustics, Interference (Learning), Stimuli, Reaction Time
Palmer, Evan M.; Horowitz, Todd S.; Torralba, Antonio; Wolfe, Jeremy M. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2011
Many visual search experiments measure response time (RT) as their primary dependent variable. Analyses typically focus on mean (or median) RT. However, given enough data, the RT distribution can be a rich source of information. For this paper, we collected about 500 trials per cell per observer for both target-present and target-absent displays…
Descriptors: Reaction Time, Cognitive Processes, Visual Perception, Experimental Psychology
Andrews, Lucy S.; Watson, Derrick G.; Humphreys, Glyn W.; Braithwaite, Jason J. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2011
Evidence for inhibitory processes in visual search comes from studies using preview conditions, where responses to new targets are delayed if they carry a featural attribute belonging to the old distractor items that are currently being ignored--the negative carry-over effect (Braithwaite, Humphreys, & Hodsoll, 2003). We examined whether…
Descriptors: Visual Perception, Inhibition, Visual Stimuli, Motion
Previous Page | Next Page »
Pages: 1 | 2