NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Showing all 10 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Nosofsky, Robert M.; Cao, Rui; Harding, Samuel M.; Shiffrin, Richard M. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2021
Participants gave recognition judgments for short lists of pictures of everyday objects. Pictures in a given list were an equal mixture of three types that varied according to the way they were used as targets and foils earlier in the same session. Under consistent-mapping (CM), targets and foils never switch roles; under varied-mapping (VM),…
Descriptors: Long Term Memory, Short Term Memory, Recognition (Psychology), Cognitive Mapping
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Cao, Rui; Nosofsky, Robert M.; Shiffrin, Richard M. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2017
In short-term-memory (STM)-search tasks, observers judge whether a test probe was present in a short list of study items. Here we investigated the long-term learning mechanisms that lead to the highly efficient STM-search performance observed under conditions of consistent-mapping (CM) training, in which targets and foils never switch roles across…
Descriptors: Short Term Memory, Recall (Psychology), Item Response Theory, Learning Processes
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Criss, Amy H.; Malmberg, Kenneth J.; Shiffrin, Richard M. – Journal of Memory and Language, 2011
Dennis and Humphreys (2001) proposed that interference in recognition memory arises solely from the prior contexts of the test word: Interference does not arise from memory traces of other words (from events prior to the study list or on the study list, and regardless of similarity to the test item). We evaluate this model using output…
Descriptors: Recognition (Psychology), Models, Context Effect
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Nelson, Angela B.; Shiffrin, Richard M. – Psychological Review, 2013
We present a theoretical framework and a simplified simulation model for the co-evolution of knowledge and event memory, both termed SARKAE (Storing and Retrieving Knowledge and Events). Knowledge is formed through the accrual of individual events, a process that operates in tandem with the storage of individual event memories. In 2 studies, new…
Descriptors: Memory, Learning, Evolution, Experience
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Nosofsky, Robert M.; Cox, Gregory E.; Cao, Rui; Shiffrin, Richard M. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2014
Experiments were conducted to test a modern exemplar-familiarity model on its ability to account for both short-term and long-term probe recognition within the same memory-search paradigm. Also, making connections to the literature on attention and visual search, the model was used to interpret differences in probe-recognition performance across…
Descriptors: Short Term Memory, Long Term Memory, Recognition (Psychology), Cognitive Processes
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Annis, Jeffrey; Malmberg, Kenneth J.; Criss, Amy H.; Shiffrin, Richard M. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2013
Recognition memory accuracy is harmed by prior testing (a.k.a., output interference [OI]; Tulving & Arbuckle, 1966). In several experiments, we interpolated various tasks between recognition test trials. The stimuli and the tasks were more similar (lexical decision [LD] of words and nonwords) or less similar (gender identification of male and…
Descriptors: Recognition (Psychology), Memory, Accuracy, Interference (Learning)
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Malmberg, Kenneth J.; Zeelenberg, Rene; Shiffrin, Richard M. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2004
E. Hirshman, J. Fisher, T. Henthom, J. Amdt, and A. Passanname (2002) found that Midazolam disrupts the mirror-patterned word-frequency effect for recognition memory by reversing the typical hit-rate advantage for low-frequency words. They noted that this result is consistent with dual-process accounts (e.g., R. C. Atkinson & J. F. Juola, 1974; G.…
Descriptors: Word Frequency, Recognition (Psychology), Memory, Cognitive Processes
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Criss, Amy H.; Shiffrin, Richard M. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2004
In studies of episodic recognition memory, low-frequency words (LF) have higher hit rates (HR) and lower false alarm rates (FAR) than do high-frequency words (HF), which is known as the mirror pattern. A few findings have suggested that requiring a task at study may reduce or eliminate the LF-HR advantage without altering the LF-FAR effect. Other…
Descriptors: Word Frequency, Language Processing, Recognition (Psychology), Memory
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Criss, Amy H.; Shiffrin, Richard M. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2005
Four experiments tested the predictions made by the model outlined in A. H. Criss and R. M. Shiffrin (2004b). Participants studied 2 successive lists of pairs followed by a recognition memory test for the most recent list. Some items and some pairs were repeated across the 2 lists. Critically, a given item could be repeated in the same or…
Descriptors: Recognition (Psychology), Experimental Psychology, Cognitive Tests, Cognitive Processes
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Gillund, Gary; Shiffrin, Richard M. – Psychological Review, 1984
The Search of Associative Memory (SAM) model for recall is extended by assuming that a familiarity process is used for recognition. The model, formalized in a computer simulation program, correctly predicts a number of findings in the literature as well as results from an experiment on the word-frequency effect. (Author/BW)
Descriptors: Association (Psychology), Computer Simulation, Cues, Memory