Descriptor
Age Differences | 45 |
Research Projects | 45 |
Gerontology | 40 |
Older Adults | 36 |
Comparative Analysis | 10 |
Males | 9 |
Behavior Patterns | 8 |
Cognitive Processes | 7 |
Educational Gerontology | 7 |
Interaction Process Analysis | 6 |
Adults | 5 |
More ▼ |
Source
Journal of Gerontology | 22 |
Educational Gerontology | 9 |
Gerontologist | 9 |
International Journal of… | 2 |
Gerontology | 1 |
Author
Costa, Paul T., Jr. | 3 |
Okun, Morris A. | 3 |
Arenberg, David | 2 |
Cicirelli, Victor G. | 2 |
McCrae, Robert R. | 2 |
Andrulis, Richard S. | 1 |
Bell, Bill D. | 1 |
Bengtson, Vern L. | 1 |
Booth, Alan | 1 |
Borges, Marilyn | 1 |
Brodzinsky, David M. | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Reports - Research | 2 |
Education Level
Audience
Location
Sweden | 1 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
Minnesota Multiphasic… | 1 |
Sixteen Personality Factor… | 1 |
Wechsler Adult Intelligence… | 1 |
What Works Clearinghouse Rating

Keitz, Suzanne M.; Gounard, Beverley Roberts – Educational Gerontology, 1976
Prior research has shown that adults generally remember pictorial stimuli better than printed words. The present study was designed to determine whether memory for these two visual modes might be differentially affected by age. These results indicate that memory processes in the elderly differ quantitatively and not qualitatively. (Author)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Cognitive Processes, Educational Gerontology, Gerontology

Winn, Francis J., Jr.; And Others – Educational Gerontology, 1976
A group of young and elderly females were tested on a paired-associate task. The results indicated that older individuals made more intrusion errors on the CVCs high in AV on the Glaze norms but low on the Archer norms. (Author)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Cognitive Development, Cognitive Processes, Educational Gerontology

Bullough, Vern; And Others – Gerontologist, 1978
A study of intellectual and creative achievers in two periods of significant intellectual and creative achievement, 18th century Scotland and 15th century Florence, found that one of the most significant variables was longevity. Achievement was more or less continuous over the whole life span. (Author)
Descriptors: Achievement, Age Differences, Creativity, Gerontology

Thomas, John C.; And Others – Journal of Gerontology, 1978
A sample of 65 healthy males (31 to 75 years old) memorized lists of six letters (familiar or unfamiliar organization). Age-related differences in the unfamiliar condition could not be wholly accounted for by psychomotor factors. The distributions of response latencies in the oldest groups had greater variance and skew. (Author)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Geriatrics, Gerontology, Males

Borges, Marilyn; Dutton, Linda J. – Gerontology, 1976
Respondents (N=532) over a wide age range rated their own lives and an "average person's life" at each of eight age intervals. Ratings varied significantly by age of target and of respondent and were increasingly differentiated as a function of both the age level being evaluated and the respondent's age. (Author)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Attitudes, Gerontology, Older Adults

Bell, Bill D. – Gerontologist, 1978
This study examines the perceived disruptive influence of a natural disaster on the individual, primary relational, and neighborhood ties of community residents. Results suggest the coping potential of the aged surpasses that of their younger counterparts. (Author)
Descriptors: Adjustment (to Environment), Age Differences, Gerontology, Older Adults

Huerta, Faye; Horton, Robert – Gerontologist, 1978
A study of the effects of the Teton Dam Disaster in 1976 suggests that elderly persons cope quite well with disaster situations and tend to report fewer adverse emotional effects and feelings of relative deprivation than younger victims. (Author)
Descriptors: Adjustment (to Environment), Age Differences, Gerontology, Older Adults

Cicirelli, Victor G. – Journal of Gerontology, 1976
Age changes in categorization and conceptual styles were studied using an object-sorting task in 276 subjects representing seven age groups. Significant age effects were found in the analysis of variance for four of six measures; there were no significant sex or interaction effects. (Author)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Behavior Patterns, Comparative Analysis, Gerontology

Arenberg, David – Journal of Gerontology, 1978
Relation of male adult age and performance on memory for designs was determined by analyzing three cross-sectional samples, two longitudinal samples, and within-cohort comparisons of men born in same period, tested at different times. Results indicate age declines in memory-for-designs performance but only small vocabulary change. (Author)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Geriatrics, Gerontology, Males

Okun, Morris A.; Siegler, Ilene C. – Educational Gerontology, 1977
Younger (N=31) and older (N=21) men participated in a bogus convergent-thinking task in which they rated their effort expenditure after experiencing varying degrees of success. As predicted, results indicated younger, but not older men perceived they tried harder when they succeeded relative to when they failed. (Author)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Cognitive Processes, Gerontology, Males

Sadowski, Bernard Stanley – Educational Gerontology, 1978
The AAAT was utilized to assess potential validity with respect to the original study by Kastenbaum and Durkee (1964). Support was found for the original conclusion that a majority of the subjects were unfavorably disposed to pastboundness of "Ted" by having perceived him as the eldest character. (Author)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Attitude Measures, Gerontology, Older Adults

Kleinman, Joel M.; Brodzinsky, David M. – Journal of Gerontology, 1978
Matching accuracy and strategy utilization in young, middle-aged, and elderly adults was examined in a series of intramodal, haptic match-to-standard problems. Results indicated that elderly adults were less successful in solving the haptic problems. They also displayed less systematic and logical haptic search strategies. (Author)
Descriptors: Adults, Age Differences, Geriatrics, Gerontology

Costa, Paul T., Jr.; McCrae, Robert R. – International Journal of Aging and Human Development, 1978
Construct validity and longitudinal stability evidence for three cluster dimensions of personality identified as Anxiety, Extraversion, and Openness is examined in a sample of adult males. Correlations with Allport-Vernon-Lindsay Value scales, Cornell Medical Index scores, Eysenck E and N scales, and factors from the SVIB are presented. (Author)
Descriptors: Adults, Age Differences, Anxiety, Cognitive Style

Giambra, Leonard M. – Gerontologist, 1977
The tendency of more than 1100 males and females aged 17-92 to daydream about the past, present, and future was determined. Contrary to common belief, no linear relation between age and daydreaming about the past was observed, and all temporal orientations were of near equal strength at all ages. (Author)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Behavior Patterns, Cognitive Processes, Gerontology

Okun, Morris A.; Elias, Cherin S. – Journal of Gerontology, 1977
Young (N=18) and older (N=18) adults participated in a vocabulary task involving varying degrees of risk with a payoff structure that varied either directly or inversely with risk. In contrast to prior research using constant payoff structures, results did not indicate that older adults are more cautious than young adults. (Author)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Behavior Patterns, Comparative Analysis, Gerontology