Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 0 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 7 |
Descriptor
Behavioral Science Research | 9 |
Brain | 9 |
Responses | 9 |
Animals | 8 |
Cues | 5 |
Stimuli | 5 |
Brain Hemisphere Functions | 4 |
Neurological Organization | 4 |
Cognitive Processes | 3 |
Conditioning | 3 |
Memory | 3 |
More ▼ |
Author
Agarwal, Isha | 1 |
Arleo, Angelo | 1 |
Brown, Kevin L. | 1 |
Burgess, Paul W. | 1 |
Burman, Michael A. | 1 |
Cerutti, D. T. | 1 |
Chavarriaga, Ricardo | 1 |
Cohen, Anna-Lisa | 1 |
Dean, Aaron M. | 1 |
Freeman, John H. | 1 |
Fuchs, Jason R. | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 9 |
Reports - Research | 7 |
Reports - Descriptive | 1 |
Reports - Evaluative | 1 |
Education Level
Audience
Location
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Brown, Kevin L.; Freeman, John H. – Learning & Memory, 2014
Eyeblink conditioning is a well-established model for studying the developmental neurobiology of associative learning and memory. However, age differences in extinction and subsequent reacquisition have yet to be studied using this model. The present study examined extinction and reacquisition of eyeblink conditioning in developing rats. In…
Descriptors: Animals, Conditioning, Neurological Organization, Associative Learning
Fuchs, Jason R.; Robinson, Gain M.; Dean, Aaron M.; Schoenberg, Heidi E.; Williams, Michael R.; Morielli, Anthony D.; Green, John T. – Learning & Memory, 2014
We have previously shown that intracerebellar infusion of the neuropeptide secretin enhances the acquisition phase of eyeblink conditioning (EBC). Here, we sought to test whether endogenous secretin also regulates EBC and to test whether the effect of exogenous and endogenous secretin is specific to acquisition. In Experiment 1, rats received…
Descriptors: Classical Conditioning, Neurological Organization, Animals, Behavioral Science Research
Gilbert, Sam J.; Gollwitzer, Peter M.; Cohen, Anna-Lisa; Burgess, Paul W.; Oettingen, Gabriele – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2009
In everyday life, one can link anticipated specific cues (e.g. visiting a restaurant) with desired actions (e.g., ordering a healthy meal). Alternatively, intentions such as "I intend to eat more healthily" present the option to act when one encounters the same cue. In the first case, a specific cue triggers a specific action; in the second, one…
Descriptors: Cues, Brain, Intention, Brain Hemisphere Functions
Jozefowiez, J.; Staddon, J. E. R.; Cerutti, D. T. – Psychological Review, 2009
The authors propose a simple behavioral economic model (BEM) describing how reinforcement and interval timing interact. The model assumes a Weber-law-compliant logarithmic representation of time. Associated with each represented time value are the payoffs that have been obtained for each possible response. At a given real time, the response with…
Descriptors: Intervals, Metacognition, Reinforcement, Time
Sheynikhovich, Denis; Chavarriaga, Ricardo; Strosslin, Thomas; Arleo, Angelo; Gerstner, Wulfram – Psychological Review, 2009
Modern psychological theories of spatial cognition postulate the existence of a geometric module for reorientation. This concept is derived from experimental data showing that in rectangular arenas with distinct landmarks in the corners, disoriented rats often make diagonal errors, suggesting their preference for the geometric (arena shape) over…
Descriptors: Cues, Spatial Ability, Geometric Concepts, Information Processing
Kerfoot, Erin C.; Agarwal, Isha; Lee, Hongjoo J.; Holland, Peter C. – Learning & Memory, 2007
Through associative learning, cues for biologically significant reinforcers such as food may gain access to mental representations of those reinforcers. Here, we used devaluation procedures, behavioral assessment of hedonic taste-reactivity responses, and measurement of immediate-early gene (IEG) expression to show that a cue for food engages…
Descriptors: Cues, Behavioral Science Research, Memory, Brain
Kasai, Yoko; Watanabe, Satoshi; Kirino, Yutaka; Matsuo, Ryota – Learning & Memory, 2006
The terrestrial slug "Limax" has a highly developed ability to associate the odor of some foods (e.g., carrot juice) with aversive stimuli such as the bitter taste of quinidine solution. The procerebrum (PC) is a part of the slug's brain thought to be involved in odor-aversion learning, but direct evidence is still lacking. Here, the authors…
Descriptors: Stimuli, Conditioning, Brain, Animals
Burman, Michael A.; Gewirtz, Jonathan C. – Learning & Memory, 2004
In two experiments, the time course of the expression of fear in trace (hippocampus-dependent) versus delay (hippocampus-independent) conditioning was characterized with a high degree of temporal specificity using fear-potentiated startle. In experiment 1, groups of rats were given delay fear conditioning or trace fear conditioning with a 3- or…
Descriptors: Stimuli, Conditioning, Fear, Anxiety
Haddon, J. E.; Killcross, A. S. – Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 2005
There is much debate as to the extent and nature of functional specialization within the different subregions of the prefrontal cortex. The current study was undertaken to investigate the effect of damage to medial prefrontal cortex subregions in the rat. Rats were trained on two biconditional discrimination tasks, one auditory and one visual, in…
Descriptors: Brain, Animals, Cues, Specialization