NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Showing all 13 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Surber, John R.; Anderson, Richard C. – Journal of Educational Psychology, 1975
The results show that the delay-retention effect occurs under conditions approximating those of real instruction and confirm the interference-perseveration interpretation of the phenomenon. (Author)
Descriptors: Comparative Analysis, Comprehension, Educational Testing, Feedback
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Spiro, Rand J.; Anderson, Richard C. – American Educational Research Journal, 1981
Ausubel asserts that his work is impugned in various ways in Anderson, Spiro, and Anderson. This paper argues that a more careful reading of the original paper obviates most of Ausubel's concerns. (Author/GK)
Descriptors: Advance Organizers, Cognitive Processes, Cognitive Style, Learning Theories
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Anderson, Richard C. – Journal of Educational Psychology, 1974
Descriptors: Associative Learning, Learning Processes, Memory, Mnemonics
Anderson, Richard C.; And Others – 1974
In two experiments a total of 662 high school students read a prose passage, took a verbatim or paraphrase quiz, and a week later completed a verbatim or paraphrase delayed test. Taking a quiz significantly enhanced performance on the delayed test. Performance was consistently much higher on the verbatim than on the paraphrase forms of quizzes and…
Descriptors: High School Students, Learning Processes, Memory, Psychological Studies
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Kulhavy, Raymond W.; Anderson, Richard C. – Journal of Educational Psychology, 1972
Evidence from the present study indicates that the delay-retention effect is due primarily to the forgetting of interference-producing errors during the delay interval and, secondarily, to the increased time a subject spends studying the feedback after a delay. (Authors)
Descriptors: Feedback, High School Students, Intervals, Multiple Choice Tests
Anderson, Richard C.; Pichert, James W. – 1977
College undergraduates read a story about two boys playing hooky from school from the perspective of either a burglar or a person interested in buying a home. After recalling the story once, subjects were directed to shift perspectives and then recall the story again. In two experiments, subjects produced on the second recall significantly more…
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, College Students, Literary Perspective, Memory
Kane, Janet Hidde; Anderson, Richard C. – 1977
In two experiments, college students who supplied the last words of sentences they read learned more than subjects who simply read whole sentences. This facilitation was observed even with a list of sentences which were almost always completed with the wrong words. However, proactive interference attributable to acquisition errors appeared on…
Descriptors: College Students, Learning Processes, Learning Theories, Memory
Pichert, James W.; Anderson, Richard C. – 1976
The two studies outlined in this report gauged college undergraduates' ability to learn and to recall the content of certain passages when provided with "directed perspectives" or context clues. In the first study, 63 subjects were divided into three groups, were asked to read two stories, and were assigned a perspective (home buyer,…
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Conceptual Schemes, Higher Education, Learning Processes
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Pichert, James W.; Anderson, Richard C. – Journal of Educational Psychology, 1977
College undergraduates read stories from one of two directed perspectives or no directed perspective. An idea's significance in terms of the assigned perspective affected both initial learning and recall one week later. Schemata, or conceptual frameworks, were assumed to aid in memory and retrieval. (Author/GDC)
Descriptors: Advance Organizers, College Students, Concept Formation, Conceptual Schemes
Freebody, Peter; Anderson, Richard C. – 1981
Multiple regression was used to examine the relationship between the serial position and the rated importance of a proposition in a text and the probability of its appearance in free recall protocols. Eight passages were used in the study--four from a standard social studies textbook and four written for the study. Each passage was divided into…
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Grade 6, Intermediate Grades, Learning Processes
Freebody, Peter; Anderson, Richard C. – 1981
Two experiments assessed the effect of vocabulary difficulty on three measures of text comprehension--free recall, summary recall, and sentence recognition. In the first experiment, the effect of differing proportions of rare-word substitutions were examined in 79 sixth grade students. It was found that a high rate of difficult vocabulary (one…
Descriptors: Difficulty Level, Grade 6, Intermediate Grades, Reading Comprehension
Anderson, Richard C.; And Others – 1974
Two experiments were conducted, the purpose of which was to investigate the direct effects of questioning. In experiment one, 240 sophomores, juniors, and seniors from a small town high school read one of two versions of a 550-word passage describing the social behavior of the army ant. The subjects then took either a verbatim or paraphrase quiz,…
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Educational Research, High School Students, Learning
Anderson, Richard C.; And Others – 1979
The effects of content schemata, embodying the reader's existing knowledge about a topic, on reading comprehension were examined in two experiments in which high school and college students were instructed to take a distinctive point of view while reading and recalling a story. Perspectives assigned before reading, shortly after reading, and long…
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Content Area Reading, Higher Education, Memory