NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Showing all 7 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Estela Garcia-Alcaraz; Juana M. Liceras – Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 2025
Unlike with the typically developing population, non-typically developing individuals, especially those with intellectual disabilities, have usually been recommended to learn and use only one language, despite perhaps coming from bilingual families or living in multilingual environments. This common practice, however, is not backed by empirical…
Descriptors: Executive Function, Bilingualism, Romance Languages, Spanish
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Mayol, Laia; Barberà, Gemma – Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 2018
The goal of this paper is to compare the different anaphoric strategies that Catalan and Catalan Sign Language (LSC) use by means of a parallel corpus. In particular, our comparison is focused in an examination of the uses of overt subject pronouns in Catalan and how these uses are rendered in a language that exploits the visual-manual modality,…
Descriptors: Romance Languages, Sign Language, Comparative Analysis, Language Usage
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Busquets, Joan – Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 2018
This paper considers the anaphoric status of the pro-form "fer-ho" (do it) in Catalan [This paper contains some ideas included in Busquets (2005)]. I discuss some anaphoric properties of "fer-ho" as deep anaphora. I also compare these properties to those of other types of anaphora, like VPE and pseudogapping (pg). I show that…
Descriptors: Romance Languages, Semantics, Comparative Analysis, Language Research
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Leonetti, Manuel – Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 2018
This paper argues against the assumption that Spanish--and more generally Romance--imperfective past (IMP) is an intrinsically anaphoric tense. It is a widely accepted view that IMP requires a temporal discourse antecedent to be licensed. My aim is to show that such requirement is not actually in force when IMP combines with a stative/atelic…
Descriptors: Grammar, Language Usage, Spanish, Sentence Structure
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Tagliapietra, Lara; Fanari, R.; Collina, S.; Tabossi, P. – Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 2009
Two cross-modal priming experiments tested whether lexical access is constrained by syllabic structure in Italian. Results extend the available Italian data on the processing of stressed syllables showing that syllabic information restricts the set of candidates to those structurally consistent with the intended word (Experiment 1). Lexical…
Descriptors: Syllables, Word Recognition, Language Processing, Romance Languages
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Gavarro, Anna; Martinez-Ferreiro, Silvia – Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 2007
We examine the inflectional productions of seven Catalan, seven Galician, and seven Spanish speaking agrammatic subjects in an elicitation and a sentence repetition task and consider them in the light of the Tree Pruning Hypothesis (TPH). The results show relatively spared subject person/number agreement with the verb and impaired tense marking…
Descriptors: Grammar, Form Classes (Languages), Morphemes, Spanish Speaking
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Diaconescu, Rodica Constanta; Goodluck, Helen – Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 2004
Frazier and Clifton (2002) argue that a d(iscourse)-linked wh-phrase such as which boy attracts the reference of a pronoun in a subordinate clause. We translated Frazier and Clifton's materials from English into Romanian. Romanian is a pro-drop language in which null subjects are licensed by person and number agreement on the verb. We found that…
Descriptors: Form Classes (Languages), Second Language Learning, Romance Languages, Native Speakers