NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Showing 1 to 15 of 23 results Save | Export
Torres, L. Valencia; Araujo, S. da Costa; Sanchez, C. M. Olarte; Body, S.; Bradshaw, C. M.; Szabadi, E. – Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 2011
Twelve rats made repeated choices on an adjusting-delay schedule between a smaller reinforcer (A) that was delivered immediately after a response and a larger reinforcer (B) that was delivered after a delay which increased or decreased by 20% depending on the subject's choices in successive blocks of trials. In two phases of the experiment (100…
Descriptors: Animals, Selection, Reinforcement, Time
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Andrade, Leonardo F.; Hackenberg, Timothy D. – Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 2012
Two experiments were conducted to compare choices between sequences of reinforcers in pigeon (Experiment 1) and human (Experiment 2) subjects, using functionally analogous procedures. The subjects made pairwise choices among 3 sequence types, all of which provided the same overall reinforcement rate, but differed in their temporal patterning.…
Descriptors: Reinforcement, Token Economy, Animals, Undergraduate Students
Rachlin, Howard; Locey, Matthew L. – Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 2010
David Thorne's (2010) article, "The Identities Hidden In The Matching Laws, And Their Uses" performs a valuable service in pointing out alternative expressions of matching. However, some identities tend to obscure rather than illuminate empirical relationships. Three such problematic instances are discussed: interresponse time as a function of…
Descriptors: Reinforcement, Probability, Selection
Schneider, Susan M.; Lickliter, Robert – Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 2010
Although newborns have surprised scientists with their learning skills, proficiency on concurrent schedules of reinforcement requires (in effect) the ability to integrate and compare behavior-consequence relations over time. Can very young animals obey the quantitative relation that applies to such repeated choices, the generalized matching law?…
Descriptors: Animals, Neonates, Reinforcement, Selection
Mattson, Karla M.; Hucks, Andrew; Grace, Randolph C.; McLean, Anthony P. – Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 2010
Eight pigeons responded in a three-component concurrent-chains procedure, with either independent or dependent initial links. Relative probability and immediacy of reinforcement in the terminal links were both varied, and outcomes on individual trials (reinforcement or nonreinforcement) were either signaled or unsignaled. Terminal-link fixed-time…
Descriptors: Reinforcement, Probability, Animals, Selection
McDevitt, Margaret A.; Williams, Ben A. – Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 2010
Pigeons were presented with a concurrent-chains schedule in which the total time to primary reinforcement was equated for the two alternatives (VI 30 s VI 60 s vs. VI 60 s VI 30 s). In one set of conditions, the terminal links were signaled by the same stimulus, and in another set of conditions they were signaled by different stimuli. Choice was…
Descriptors: Stimuli, Reinforcement, Conditioning, Animals
Christensen, Darren R.; Grace, Randolph C. – Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 2010
Grace and McLean (2006) proposed a decision model for acquisition of choice in concurrent chains which assumes that after reinforcement in a terminal link, subjects make a discrimination whether the preceding reinforcer delay was short or long relative to a criterion. Their model was subsequently extended by Christensen and Grace (2008, 2009a,…
Descriptors: Selection, Decision Making, Models, Conditioning
White, K. Geoffrey; Brown, Glenn S. – Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 2011
Pigeons performed a delayed matching-to-sample task in which large or small reinforcers for correct remembering were signaled during the retention interval. Accuracy was low when small reinforcers were signaled, and high when large reinforcers were signaled (the signaled magnitude effect). When the reinforcer-size cue was switched from small to…
Descriptors: Animals, Reinforcement, Accuracy, Memory
Stilling, Stephanie T.; Critchfield, Thomas S. – Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 2010
The utility of a quantitative model depends on the extent to which its fitted parameters vary systematically with environmental events of interest. Professional football statistics were analyzed to determine whether play selection (passing versus rushing plays) could be accounted for with the generalized matching equation, and in particular…
Descriptors: Play, Team Sports, Probability, Bias
Lie, Celia; Alsop, Brent – Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 2009
Three experiments using human participants varied the distribution of point-gain reinforcers or point-loss punishers in two-alternative signal-detection procedures. Experiment 1 varied the distribution of point-gain reinforcers for correct responses (Group A) and point-loss punishers for errors (Group B) across conditions. Response bias varied…
Descriptors: Positive Reinforcement, Bias, Response Style (Tests), Punishment
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Ta, Wei-Min; Pitts, Raymond C.; Hughes, Christine E.; McLean, Anthony P.; Grace, Randolph C. – Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 2008
The purpose of this study was to examine effects of "d"-amphetamine on choice controlled by reinforcement delay. Eight pigeons responded under a concurrent-chains procedure in which one terminal-link schedule was always fixed- interval 8 s, and the other terminal-link schedule changed from session to session between fixed-interval 4 s and…
Descriptors: Preferences, Reinforcement, Intervals, Time
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Rasmussen, Erin B.; Newland, M. Christopher – Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 2008
The hypothesis that a penny lost is valued more highly than a penny earned was tested in human choice. Five participants clicked a computer mouse under concurrent variable-interval schedules of monetary reinforcement. In the no-punishment condition, the schedules arranged monetary gain. In the punishment conditions, a schedule of monetary loss was…
Descriptors: Reinforcement, Punishment, Selection, Behavior
Fantino, Edmund; Kennelly, Arthur – Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 2009
Students chose between two allocation options, one that gave the allocator more and another participant still more (the "optimal" choice) and one which gave the allocator less and the other participant still less (the "competitive" choice). In a within-subjects design, students' behavior patterns were significantly correlated across the two rounds…
Descriptors: Student Behavior, Resource Allocation, Sharing Behavior, Behavior Patterns
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Mazur, James E. – Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 2007
Pigeons responded in a successive-encounters procedure that consisted of a search state, a choice state, and a handling state. The search state was either a fixed-interval or mixed-interval schedule presented on the center key of a three-key chamber. Upon completion of the search state, the choice state was presented, in which the center key was…
Descriptors: Selection, Reinforcement, Animals, Intervals
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Fantino, Edmund; Romanowich, Paul – Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 2007
We review the nature of conditioned reinforcement, including evidence that conditioned reinforcers maintain choice behavior in concurrent schedules and that they elevate responding in the terminal links of concurrent-chains schedules. A question has resurfaced recently: Do theories of choice in concurrent-chains schedules need to include a term…
Descriptors: Reinforcement, Conditioning, Selection, Preferences
Previous Page | Next Page ยป
Pages: 1  |  2