NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Publication Date
In 20250
Since 20240
Since 2021 (last 5 years)0
Since 2016 (last 10 years)0
Since 2006 (last 20 years)5
Audience
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Showing 1 to 15 of 68 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Burnett, Debra L. – Journal of Child Language, 2015
Irony comprehension in seven- and eight-year-old children with typically developing language skills was explored under the framework of the graded salience hypothesis. Target ironic remarks, either conventional or novel/situation-specific, were presented following brief story contexts. Children's responses to comprehension questions were used to…
Descriptors: Child Language, Young Children, Figurative Language, Comprehension
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Thomsen, Ditte Boeg; Poulsen, Mads – Journal of Child Language, 2015
When learning their first language, children develop strategies for assigning semantic roles to sentence structures, depending on morphosyntactic cues such as case and word order. Traditionally, comprehension experiments have presented transitive clauses in isolation, and cross-linguistically children have been found to misinterpret object-first…
Descriptors: Foreign Countries, Child Language, Indo European Languages, Preschool Children
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Pyykkonen, Pirita; Matthews, Danielle; Jarvikivi, Juhani – Language and Cognitive Processes, 2010
Recent evidence from adult pronoun comprehension suggests that semantic factors such as verb transitivity affect referent salience and thereby anaphora resolution. We tested whether the same semantic factors influence pronoun comprehension in young children. In a visual world study, 3-year-olds heard stories that began with a sentence containing…
Descriptors: Comprehension, Sentences, Semantics, Verbs
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Soderstrom, Melanie – Journal of Child Language, 2008
Two recent papers (de Villiers & Johnson, 2007; Johnson, de Villiers & Seymour, 2005) have claimed that children have difficulty with verbal "-s" until five- six-years-old. This contrasts with perceptual studies showing evidence for sensitivity to the grammatical properties of verbal "-s" as young as 1;4. These apparently conflicting findings can…
Descriptors: Semantics, Grammar, Child Language, Language Acquisition
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Berent, Gerald P.; Kelly, Ronald R.; Schueler-Choukairi, Tanya – Applied Psycholinguistics, 2009
English sentences containing the universal quantifiers "each", "every", and "all" are highly complex structures in view of the subtleties of their scope properties and resulting ambiguities. This study explored the acquisition of universal quantifier sentences as reflected in the performance of three diverse college-level student groups on a…
Descriptors: Sentences, Semantics, Nouns, Deafness
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Charney, Rosalind – Journal of Child Language, 1979
Reports on an experiment, performed on seven children, designed to show that children understand "here" and "there" with the self as reference point before they understand words such as these with reference to other speakers as reference points. (Author/AM)
Descriptors: Child Language, Cognitive Processes, Comprehension, Egocentrism
Gowie, Cheryl Janice – 1973
This study examined the extent of children's awareness of the semantic subtleties of the word "promise" and their comprehension of sentences following an atypical syntactic pattern using "promise" as the main verb. Subjects included children within three months of being six-and-one-half, eight-and-one-half, nine-and-one-half, and ten-and-one-half…
Descriptors: Child Language, Comprehension, Expectation, Language Research
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Bartlett, Elsa Jaffe – Journal of Child Language, 1976
Two tasks were used to test predictions of the Semantic Feature Hypothesis (SFH) about children's comprehension of the meaning of spatial adjectives. Predictions about acquisition order for dimensional features were supported; predictions about polarity were not. An acquisition hypothesis is offered that is contrary to the SFH. (Author/RM)
Descriptors: Adjectives, Child Language, Comprehension, Hypothesis Testing
Weist, Richard M.; Stebbins, Pat – Psychonomic Science, 1972
Research supported by a University of Nebraska Research Council Summer Fellowship. (DD)
Descriptors: Adults, Bilingualism, Child Language, Comprehension
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Fritz, Janet J.; Suci, George J. – Journal of Child Language, 1982
Research results show that it may be possible, within limitations, to facilitate discrimination by infants of inappropriate from appropriate verbal descriptions of a visual event, by emphasizing the agent component in a simple sentence. (Author/JB)
Descriptors: Child Language, Comprehension, Infants, Language Acquisition
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Tager-Flusberg, Helen – Applied Psycholinguistics, 1981
Comprehension and strategy use of 18 autistic children was compared with that of normal 3- and 4-year olds. Subjects were asked to act out certain syntactic and semantic patterns in two experiments. Autistic children performed below the levels of the normal subjects, suggesting that autism is a semantic/cognitive deficit. (PJM)
Descriptors: Autism, Child Language, Comprehension, Language Handicaps
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Reeder, Kenneth – Journal of Child Language, 1980
An experiment was designed to answer the following: (1) can children as young as 2 1/2 to 3 years of age employ contextual cues in order to distinguish request from offers? and (2) do children's discrimination skills for these illocutionary acts improve with age? A model of the comprehension of illocutionary force is proposed. (Author/AM)
Descriptors: Child Language, Comprehension, Language Acquisition, Language Research
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Wilcox, Stephen; Palermo, David S. – Cognition, 1974
Presents evidence that young children's comprehension of the locatives "in", "on", and "under" is, at least in part, contextually determined. Children were given tasks with verbal instructions which were either contextually congruent or incongruent. Results are interpreted in terms of the non-linguistic as well as linguistic strategies apparently…
Descriptors: Child Language, Cognitive Processes, Comprehension, Language Acquisition
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Corrigan, Roberta; Odya-Weis, Cyndie – Journal of Child Language, 1985
Discusses a study that examines which combination of animate and inanimate actors (anyone or anything performing an action) and patients (the thing that is the object of action) two-year-olds view as prototypical. Results suggest that the actor category is usually acquired first for prototypical sentences with animate actors and inanimate…
Descriptors: Child Language, Comprehension, Language Acquisition, Language Processing
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Astington, Janet W. – Journal of Child Language, 1988
A study determined what types of speech act five- to 13-year-olds and adults would define as "promising." Results indicate that children could distinguish between "promising" and "predicting" in terms of the speaker's responsibility for the outcome. (Author/CB)
Descriptors: Adults, Child Language, Childhood Attitudes, Children
Previous Page | Next Page ยป
Pages: 1  |  2  |  3  |  4  |  5