Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 2 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 3 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 6 |
Descriptor
Comprehension | 15 |
Sentence Structure | 15 |
Word Order | 15 |
Verbs | 6 |
Language Acquisition | 5 |
Preschool Children | 5 |
Syntax | 5 |
Child Language | 4 |
Grammar | 4 |
Language Research | 4 |
Semantics | 4 |
More ▼ |
Source
Author
Bastiaanse, Roelien | 2 |
Lempert, Henrietta | 2 |
Cheng, Qi | 1 |
Edwards, Susan | 1 |
Evans, Julia L. | 1 |
Frankel, Daniel G. | 1 |
Galang, Rosita G. | 1 |
Gillam, Ronald B. | 1 |
Hakuta, Kenji | 1 |
Hale, John | 1 |
Harris, Margaret | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 9 |
Reports - Research | 9 |
Reports - Descriptive | 2 |
Dissertations/Theses -… | 1 |
Reports - Evaluative | 1 |
Education Level
Early Childhood Education | 1 |
Kindergarten | 1 |
Primary Education | 1 |
Audience
Location
Denmark | 1 |
Israel | 1 |
Philippines | 1 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Valerie Johanne Langlois – ProQuest LLC, 2021
Comprehenders encounter a variety of syntactic structures through reading or spoken conversation. In some cases, sentences can be ambiguous and have more than one meaning. In "The spy saw the cop with the binoculars," one interpretation is that the spy is looking through the binoculars, while an alternative is that the cop has the…
Descriptors: Comprehension, Pacing, Verbs, Syntax
Cheng, Qi; Mayberry, Rachel I. – Developmental Science, 2021
Limited language experience in childhood is common among deaf individuals, which prior research has shown to lead to low levels of language processing. Although basic structures such as word order have been found to be resilient to conditions of sparse language input in early life, whether they are robust to conditions of extreme language delay is…
Descriptors: Word Order, Sentence Structure, Sentences, Comprehension
Montgomery, James W.; Gillam, Ronald B.; Evans, Julia L.; Sergeev, Alexander V. – Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 2017
Purpose: With Aim 1, we compared the comprehension of and sensitivity to canonical and noncanonical word order structures in school-age children with specific language impairment (SLI) and same-age typically developing (TD) children. Aim 2 centered on the developmental improvement of sentence comprehension in the groups. With Aim 3, we compared…
Descriptors: Sentences, Comprehension, Language Impairments, Children
Thomsen, Ditte Boeg; Poulsen, Mads – Journal of Child Language, 2015
When learning their first language, children develop strategies for assigning semantic roles to sentence structures, depending on morphosyntactic cues such as case and word order. Traditionally, comprehension experiments have presented transitive clauses in isolation, and cross-linguistically children have been found to misinterpret object-first…
Descriptors: Foreign Countries, Child Language, Indo European Languages, Preschool Children
Hale, John – Cognitive Science, 2006
A word-by-word human sentence processing complexity metric is presented. This metric formalizes the intuition that comprehenders have more trouble on words contributing larger amounts of information about the syntactic structure of the sentence as a whole. The formalization is in terms of the conditional entropy of grammatical continuations, given…
Descriptors: Sentences, Sentence Structure, Grammar, Prediction
Bastiaanse, Roelien; van Zonneveld, Ron – Brain and Language, 2006
Drai and Grodzinsky have statistically analyzed a large corpus of data on the comprehension of passives by patients with Broca's aphasia. The data come, according to Drai and Grodzinsky, from binary choice tasks. Among the languages that are analyzed are Dutch and German. Drai and Grodzinsky argue that Dutch and German speaking Broca patients…
Descriptors: Patients, Aphasia, Comprehension, Indo European Languages

Lempert, Henrietta; Kinsbourne, Marcel – Child Development, 1978
Children ranging in age from three to seven years acted out the meaning of reversible active, cleft, passive, and inverted cleft sentences. (JMB)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Comprehension, Elementary School Students, Preschool Children
Bastiaanse, Roelien; Edwards, Susan – Brain and Language, 2004
The effect of two linguistic factors in Broca's and Wernicke's aphasia was examined using Dutch and English subjects. Three tasks were used to test (1) the comprehension and (2) the construction of sentences, where verbs (in Dutch) and verb arguments (in Dutch and English) are in canonical versus non-canonical position; (3) the production of…
Descriptors: Indo European Languages, Verbs, Word Order, Speech Impairments
Revlin, Russell; Kallio, Kenneth – 1981
The reversal of subject and predicate terms in quantified, categorical expressions was studied as an operation that is potentially important in issues of representation and comprehension of quantified relations. In two experiments students were asked to evaluate the relation between two quantified expressions. The salience of reversal in the…
Descriptors: Comprehension, Context Clues, Higher Education, Language Patterns

van der Lely, Heather K. J.; Harris, Margaret – Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 1990
Fourteen specifically language-impaired children, age four to seven, pointed to pictures in, and acted out, semantically reversible sentences that varied in thematic content and in the order of thematic roles. Compared to children matched on language age and chronological age, subjects' comprehension was significantly lower. (Author/JDD)
Descriptors: Comprehension, Early Childhood Education, Language Acquisition, Language Handicaps

Segalowitz, Norman S.; Galang, Rosita G. – Journal of Child Language, 1978
In a study, Tagalog-speaking children, 3-, 5-, and 7-year olds, demonstrated better mastery of patient-focus (passive) than agent-focus (active) sentence structure. These results were attributed to the children's strategy of interpreting the first noun of a sentence to be the agent of the action. (SW)
Descriptors: Child Language, Cognitive Processes, Comprehension, Intellectual Development
McClelland, James L.; Kawamoto, Alan H. – 1986
This paper describes and illustrates a simulation model for the processing of grammatical elements in a sentence, focusing on one aspect of sentence comprehension: the assignment of the constituent elements of a sentence to the correct thematic case roles. The model addresses questions about sentence processing from a perspective very different…
Descriptors: Case (Grammar), Comparative Analysis, Comprehension, Computational Linguistics
Hakuta, Kenji – 1977
Comprehension of reversible active and passive sentences was studied with 48 Japanese children between the ages of two and six. Four types of sentences were constructed using passive and active structures and two word orders: subject-object-verb (SOV) and object-subject-verb (OSV). The basic order of elements in a simple sentence in Japanese is…
Descriptors: Child Language, Cognitive Development, Comprehension, Grammar

Frankel, Daniel G.; And Others – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1980
Monolingual Hebrew-speaking subjects aged 3, 4, 5, 6, 7, and 11 years, as well as college students, were asked to interpret utterances consisting of two nouns and a verb in order to determine whether Hebrew speakers can rely on a word order strategy to assign sentence relation. (MP)
Descriptors: Age Differences, College Students, Comprehension, Elementary Education
Lempert, Henrietta – 1981
Preschoolers' ability to understand grammatical relations in passives and to generalize was studied using animate referents. Three- to five-year-old children were taught to produce passive sentence descriptions of events in which animacy of the actor and acted-on object were varied. After pretesting to determine passive sentence comprehension, the…
Descriptors: Case (Grammar), Child Language, Comprehension, Concept Formation