Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 10 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 37 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 59 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 87 |
Descriptor
Morphemes | 129 |
Sentence Structure | 129 |
Grammar | 61 |
Verbs | 51 |
Syntax | 36 |
Language Processing | 35 |
Form Classes (Languages) | 34 |
Semantics | 31 |
Language Research | 29 |
Linguistic Theory | 28 |
Language Acquisition | 26 |
More ▼ |
Source
Author
Crain, Stephen | 3 |
Bock, Kathryn | 2 |
Breheny, Richard | 2 |
Clahsen, Harald | 2 |
Grodzinsky, Yosef | 2 |
Guasti, Maria Teresa | 2 |
Kaup, Barbara | 2 |
Lungu, Oana | 2 |
Moscati, Vincenzo | 2 |
Pagliarini, Elena | 2 |
Sang-Gu Kang | 2 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Education Level
Higher Education | 15 |
Postsecondary Education | 14 |
Elementary Education | 5 |
Early Childhood Education | 3 |
Primary Education | 3 |
Kindergarten | 2 |
Adult Education | 1 |
Grade 1 | 1 |
Grade 12 | 1 |
Grade 7 | 1 |
Grade 8 | 1 |
More ▼ |
Audience
Students | 3 |
Practitioners | 2 |
Teachers | 2 |
Community | 1 |
Location
Indonesia | 3 |
Netherlands | 3 |
Australia | 2 |
Italy | 2 |
Japan | 2 |
South Africa | 2 |
Thailand | 2 |
Arkansas | 1 |
California | 1 |
California (San Jose) | 1 |
France | 1 |
More ▼ |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
National Defense Education… | 1 |
Assessments and Surveys
Preschool Language Scale | 1 |
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Huiyu Wang; Ying Wei; Mingxin Yao – Written Communication, 2024
Researchers' investment in reader engagement includes the construction of an appealing abstract. While numerous studies have been conducted on abstracts' rhetorical features, scant empirical attention has been paid to negation use in academic writing. The current study seeks to narrow the research gap from a general and diachronic perspective by…
Descriptors: Science Education, Writing (Composition), Documentation, Academic Language
Sang-Gu Kang – Journal of Pan-Pacific Association of Applied Linguistics, 2024
This paper reports on a young Korean boy's target-like and non-target-like uses of the Korean negation marker "ani" to express various types of negation in Korean, observed approximately between the ages of 2;2 and 2;5. Besides the target-like usage of "ani" as a sentential adverb for a 'no' response, he used "ani" in…
Descriptors: Korean, Morphemes, Toddlers, Language Acquisition
Sang-Gu Kang – Journal of Pan-Pacific Association of Applied Linguistics, 2024
This paper reports on a young Korean boy's target-like and non-target-like uses of the Korean negation marker "ani" to express various types of negation in Korean, observed approximately between the ages of 2;2 and 2;5. Besides the target-like usage of "ani" as a sentential adverb for a 'no' response, he used "ani" in…
Descriptors: Korean, Morphemes, Toddlers, Language Acquisition
Michael Hermann Hahn – ProQuest LLC, 2022
As humans, we use language with ease and speed, solving the complex computational problem of processing form and meaning seemingly without effort. This dissertation studies how the properties of language enable us to achieve this, by investigating what is computationally difficult about language, and what is easy. We first investigate the…
Descriptors: Language Usage, Difficulty Level, Artificial Intelligence, Language Processing
Meilin Zhan; Sihan Chen; Roger Levy; Jiayi Lu; Edward Gibson – Cognitive Science, 2023
Previous work has shown that English native speakers interpret sentences as predicted by a noisy-channel model: They integrate both the real-world plausibility of the meaning--the prior--and the likelihood that the intended sentence may be corrupted into the perceived sentence. In this study, we test the noisy-channel model in Mandarin Chinese, a…
Descriptors: Sentences, Mandarin Chinese, Native Language, Sentence Structure
Sara J. Margolin; Timothy Brackins – Educational Gerontology, 2024
Negated text is a difficult text construction that readers encounter in various forms throughout their lives. Despite a wealth of research on its impact, including potential strategies to improve comprehension, readers maintain poor comprehension when encountering this text construction. Given its large potential impact on reading texts like…
Descriptors: Older Adults, Reading Comprehension, Reading Strategies, Accuracy
Hannah Sawyer; Colin Bannard; Julian Pine – Developmental Science, 2024
There is substantial evidence that children's apparent omission of grammatical morphemes in utterances such as "She play tennis" and "Mummy eating" is in fact errors of commission in which contextually licensed unmarked forms encountered in the input are reproduced in a context-blind fashion. So how do children stop making such…
Descriptors: Verbs, Computational Linguistics, Preschool Children, Grammar
Wang, Shenshen; Sun, Chao; Tian, Ye; Breheny, Richard – Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 2021
In the long history of psycholinguistic research on verifying negative sentences, an often-reported finding is that participants take longer to correctly judge negative sentences true than false, while being faster to judge their positive counterparts true (e.g. Clark & Chase, Cogn Psychol 3(3):472-517, 1972; Carpenter & Just, Psychol Rev…
Descriptors: Psycholinguistics, Morphemes, Language Processing, Sentence Structure
Mabel L. Rice; Kathleen Kelsey Earnest; Lesa Hoffman – Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 2023
Purpose: Identification of children with specific language impairment (SLI) can be difficult even though their language can lag that of age peers throughout childhood. A clinical grammar marker featuring tense marking in simple clauses is valid and reliable for young children but is limited by ceiling effects around the age of 8 years. This study…
Descriptors: Morphemes, Difficulty Level, Language Impairments, Children
Yoshiki Fujiwara; Hiroyuki Shimada – Language Acquisition: A Journal of Developmental Linguistics, 2024
The goal of this paper is to tease apart two approaches to the source of children's consistent scope assignment in negative sentences containing logical connectives: the Semantic Subset Principle and the Semantic Subset Maxim. Previous developmental work has observed that four- to six-year-old children across languages have difficulty with…
Descriptors: Semantics, Language Acquisition, Form Classes (Languages), Morphemes
Gulgowski, Piotr; Blaszczak, Joanna – Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 2020
The number meaning of grammatically plural nouns is to some extent context sensitive. In negative sentences, plural nouns typically receive an inclusive reading referring to any number of individuals (one or many). This contrasts with their more frequent exclusive reading referring to a group of two or more individuals. The present study…
Descriptors: Morphemes, Nouns, Polish, Psycholinguistics
Gu, Wenyuan – Online Submission, 2020
The use of present and past participles and gerunds was summarized and illustrated from various examples cited or given, on the basis of the writer's teaching experience, and extensive review of different English grammar books, reference books, magazines, newspapers, books, English dictionaries, and online articles, in order for English language…
Descriptors: Grammar, Form Classes (Languages), Verbs, Nouns
Orenes, Isabel – Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 2021
Many studies have shown the double processing of negation, suggesting that negation integration into sentence meaning is delayed. This contrasts with some researches that have found that such integration is rather immediate. The present study contributes to this debate. Affirmative and negative compound sentences (e.g., "because he was…
Descriptors: Language Processing, Psycholinguistics, Eye Movements, Morphemes
Davies, Benjamin; Xu Rattansone, Nan; Demuth, Katherine – Journal of Child Language, 2020
Subject-verb (SV) agreement helps listeners interpret the number condition of ambiguous nouns ("The sheep is/are fat"), yet it remains unclear whether young children use agreement to comprehend newly encountered nouns. Preschoolers and adults completed a forced choice task where sentences contained singular vs. plural copulas…
Descriptors: Preschool Children, Verbs, Nouns, Grammar
Liu, Mingya; Barthel, Mathias – Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 2021
In this paper, the meaning and processing of the German conditional connectives (CCs) such as "wenn" 'if' and "nur wenn" 'only if' are investigated. In Experiment 1, participants read short scenarios containing a conditional sentence (i.e., If P, Q.) with "wenn"/"nur wenn" 'if/only if' and a confirmed or…
Descriptors: German, Language Processing, Psycholinguistics, Morphemes