Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 0 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 5 |
Descriptor
Brain | 5 |
Sentence Structure | 5 |
Sentences | 5 |
Language Processing | 4 |
Semantics | 3 |
Adults | 2 |
Word Order | 2 |
Aphasia | 1 |
Children | 1 |
Classification | 1 |
Comparative Analysis | 1 |
More ▼ |
Author
Weber-Fox, Christine | 2 |
James, Lori E. | 1 |
Kemmerer, David | 1 |
Lau, Ellen | 1 |
MacKay, Donald G. | 1 |
Malaia, Evguenia | 1 |
Marian, Diane E. | 1 |
Phillips, Colin | 1 |
Plesch, Silke | 1 |
Price, Cathy J. | 1 |
Price, Karen | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 5 |
Reports - Research | 4 |
Reports - Descriptive | 1 |
Education Level
Audience
Location
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Richardson, Fiona M.; Thomas, Michael S. C.; Price, Cathy J. – Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 2010
Semantically reversible sentences are prone to misinterpretation and take longer for typically developing children and adults to comprehend; they are also particularly problematic for those with language difficulties such as aphasia or Specific Language Impairment. In our study, we used fMRI to compare the processing of semantically reversible and…
Descriptors: Sentences, Semantics, Sentence Structure, Language Impairments
Malaia, Evguenia; Wilbur, Ronnie B.; Weber-Fox, Christine – Brain and Language, 2009
Verbs contain multifaceted information about both the semantics of an action, and potential argument structures. Linguistic theory classifies verbs according to whether the denoted action has an inherent (telic) end-point ("fall," "awaken"), or whether it is considered homogenous, or atelic ("read," "worship"). The aim of our study was to examine…
Descriptors: Sentences, Semantics, Sentence Structure, Verbs
Kemmerer, David; Weber-Fox, Christine; Price, Karen; Zdanczyk, Cynthia; Way, Heather – Brain and Language, 2007
Event-related brain potentials (ERPs) were recorded while participants read and made acceptability judgments about sentences containing three types of adjective sequences: (1) normal sequences--e.g., "Jennifer rode a huge gray elephant"; (2) reversed sequences that violate grammatical-semantic constraints on linear order--e.g., *"Jennifer rode a…
Descriptors: Form Classes (Languages), Sentences, Semantics, Sentence Structure
Lau, Ellen; Stroud, Clare; Plesch, Silke; Phillips, Colin – Brain and Language, 2006
A number of recent electrophysiological studies of sentence processing have shown that a subclass of syntactic violations elicits very rapid ERP responses, occurring within around 200 ms of the onset of the violation. Such findings raise the question of how it is possible to diagnose violations so quickly. This paper suggests that very rapid…
Descriptors: Language Processing, Sentences, Word Order, Sentence Structure
MacKay, Donald G.; James, Lori E.; Taylor, Jennifer K.; Marian, Diane E. – Language and Cognitive Processes, 2007
This study examines sentence-level language abilities of amnesic H.M. to test competing theoretical conceptions of relations between language and memory. We present 11 new sources of experimental evidence indicating deficits in H.M's comprehension and production of non-cliche sentences. Contrary to recent claims that H.M.'s comprehension is…
Descriptors: Language Processing, Sentences, Sentence Structure, Grammar