Publication Date
In 2025 | 2 |
Since 2024 | 10 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 13 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 14 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 91 |
Descriptor
Shift Studies | 242 |
Discrimination Learning | 64 |
Responses | 35 |
Cognitive Processes | 32 |
Learning Processes | 30 |
Age Differences | 26 |
Preschool Children | 26 |
Cues | 25 |
Elementary School Students | 24 |
Mediation Theory | 21 |
Attention | 20 |
More ▼ |
Source
Author
Publication Type
Education Level
Audience
Researchers | 3 |
Practitioners | 2 |
Media Staff | 1 |
Location
California | 2 |
Canada | 2 |
China | 2 |
New Zealand | 2 |
Australia | 1 |
Canada (Toronto) | 1 |
Hong Kong | 1 |
Iowa | 1 |
Japan | 1 |
Malaysia | 1 |
Mexico | 1 |
More ▼ |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
No Child Left Behind Act 2001 | 1 |
Assessments and Surveys
Metropolitan Readiness Tests | 1 |
Stanford Achievement Tests | 1 |
Trail Making Test | 1 |
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Rickard, Timothy C.; Bajic, Daniel – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2006
The applicability of the identical elements (IE) model of arithmetic fact retrieval (T. C. Rickard, A. F. Healy, & L. E. Bourne, 1994) to cued recall from episodic (image and sentence) memory was explored in 3 transfer experiments. In agreement with results from arithmetic, speedup following even minimal practice recalling a missing word from an…
Descriptors: Cues, Recall (Psychology), Visual Stimuli, Sentences

Geis, Mary Fulcher – Developmental Psychology, 1975
Second and sixth grade children's relative sensitivity to acoustic, semantic, and physical dimensions was inferred from the amount of release from proactive interference obtained for shifts along each dimension. (ED)
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Elementary Education, Elementary School Students, Memory

Pruitt, Dean G.; Cosentino, Charles – Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 1975
The first study in this article attempted to provide an empirical assessment of the validity of the other-self and self-ideal indices as measures of value strength, while the second study evaluated the assumptions that item-specific values underlie the choice shift, with an across-people (group-composition) design. (Author/RK)
Descriptors: Group Norms, Risk, Shift Studies, Social Psychology

Brown, Ann L.; Campione, Joseph C. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1973
Response to PS 502 660. (CB)
Descriptors: Discrimination Learning, Learning Theories, Mediation Theory, Research Methodology

Berman, Phyllis W. – Child Development, 1973
If learning is viewed in terms of the tendency to approach a stimulus that has been rewarded and to avoid a stimulus that has not been rewarded, then it must be concluded that the subjects in this study did not learn. (Author)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Discrimination Learning, Preschool Children, Responses
Spetch, Marcia L.; Cheng, Ken; Clifford, Colin W. G. – Learning and Motivation, 2004
University students were trained to discriminate between two gray-scale images of faces that varied along a continuum from a unique face to an average face created by morphing. Following training, participants were tested without feedback for their ability to recognize the positive face (S+) within a range of faces along the continuum. In…
Descriptors: College Students, Stimuli, Experiments, Visual Discrimination
Cates, Gary L.; Erkfritz, Karyn N. – Psychology in the Schools, 2007
The current study investigated the discreet task completion hypothesis presented by C. H. Skinner (2002) by investigating how the rate of interspersing affects performance on and preferences for academic assignments. Specifically, 70 sixth-, seventh-, and eighth-grade students were presented with four assignment pairs of multiplication problems.…
Descriptors: Multiplication, Assignments, Performance Based Assessment, Student Attitudes
Perruchet, Pierre; Cleeremans, Axel; Destrebecqz, Arnaud – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2006
After repeated associations between two events, E1 and E2, responses to E2 can be facilitated either because participants consciously expect E2 to occur after E1 or because E1 automatically activates the response to E2, or because of both. In this article, the authors report on 4 experiments designed to pit the influence of these 2 factors against…
Descriptors: Reaction Time, Influences, Expectation, Associative Learning
Reed, Catherine L.; Grubb, Jefferson D.; Steele, Cleophus – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2006
This study explored whether hand location affected spatial attention. The authors used a visual covert-orienting paradigm to examine whether spatial attention mechanisms--location prioritization and shifting attention--were supported by bimodal, hand-centered representations of space. Placing 1 hand next to a target location, participants detected…
Descriptors: Cues, Needs Assessment, Spatial Ability, Attention
Steinhauser, Marco; Hubner, Ronald – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2006
The hypothesis is introduced that 1 source of shift costs is the strengthening of task-related associations occurring whenever an overt response is produced. The authors tested this account by examining shift effects following errors and error compensation processes. The authors predicted that following a specific type of error, called task…
Descriptors: Responses, Error Correction, Association (Psychology), Task Analysis
Flom, Ross; Bahrick, Lorraine E. – Developmental Psychology, 2007
This research examined the developmental course of infants' ability to perceive affect in bimodal (audiovisual) and unimodal (auditory and visual) displays of a woman speaking. According to the intersensory redundancy hypothesis (L. E. Bahrick, R. Lickliter, & R. Flom, 2004), detection of amodal properties is facilitated in multimodal stimulation…
Descriptors: Stimulation, Social Development, Redundancy, Infants
Levy, Gary D.; Carter, D. Bruce – 1989
This study focused on the influence of gender schemas on children's abilities to focus their attention away from or toward stimuli containing the dimension of gender. Children identified as gender schematic and aschematic participated in a nonreversal discrimination learning paradigm in which one relevant dimension was gender-relevant and another…
Descriptors: Attention, Discrimination Learning, Preschool Children, Preschool Education

Daehler, Marvin W.; And Others – Child Development, 1976
This study examined the equivalence of objects and pictures of objects in transfer discrimination of 72 children (ages 24-45 months). (BRT)
Descriptors: Discrimination Learning, Infants, Perception, Preschool Children

Ozioko, Julius O. C. – International Journal of Behavioral Development, 1983
A total of 72 Nigerian (Igbo) children learned reversal or extradimensional shifts under one of two conditions of locality (urban or rural) and one of three conditions of age (4, 7, or 10 years). Results showed significant main effects for shift, age, and locality and were interpreted as being consistent with the Kendlers' (1962) mediational…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Foreign Countries, Mediation Theory, Rural Urban Differences
Enkvist, Tommy; Newell, Ben; Juslin, Peter; Olsson, Henrik – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2006
Previous studies have suggested better learning when people actively intervene rather than when they passively observe the stimuli in a judgment task. In 4 experiments, the authors investigated the hypothesis that this improvement is associated with a shift from exemplar memory to cue abstraction. In a multiple-cue judgment task with continuous…
Descriptors: Intervention, Cues, Learning Processes, Memory