Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 0 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 1 |
Descriptor
Spatial Ability | 20 |
Memory | 16 |
Cognitive Processes | 7 |
Adults | 5 |
Mental Retardation | 5 |
Short Term Memory | 5 |
Age Differences | 4 |
Cognitive Ability | 4 |
Elementary Education | 4 |
Learning Disabilities | 4 |
Recall (Psychology) | 4 |
More ▼ |
Source
Author
Publication Type
Reports - Research | 17 |
Journal Articles | 15 |
Speeches/Meeting Papers | 4 |
Books | 1 |
Collected Works - General | 1 |
Information Analyses | 1 |
Reports - Descriptive | 1 |
Reports - Evaluative | 1 |
Education Level
Audience
Researchers | 20 |
Practitioners | 1 |
Location
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
Kaufman Assessment Battery… | 1 |
Wechsler Intelligence Scale… | 1 |
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Berg, Derek H. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2008
The cognitive underpinnings of arithmetic calculation in children are noted to involve working memory; however, cognitive processes related to arithmetic calculation and working memory suggest that this relationship is more complex than stated previously. The purpose of this investigation was to examine the relative contributions of processing…
Descriptors: Short Term Memory, Computation, Cognitive Processes, Arithmetic

Herman, James F.; And Others – Developmental Psychology, 1987
Eight-, eleven-, and nineteen-year-olds' memory for spatial locations over an extended time period was assessed. Study suggested that adults remember spatial location information better than children over time because adults code location information in more organized representations and use better retrieval cues. (RWB)
Descriptors: Adolescents, Adults, Age Differences, Children
Rosenquist, Celia; Conners, Frances A.; Roskos-Ewoldsen, Beverly – American Journal on Mental Retardation, 2003
Differences in storage and rehearsal components of the phonological loop and visuo-spatial sketchpad were investigated in individuals with (n=19) and without (n=21) intellectual disability matched on memory span. Those with intellectual disability had specific difficulty in the rehearsal component of the phonological loop. Groups did not differ in…
Descriptors: Adolescents, Cognitive Processes, Drills (Practice), Memorization
Hanley, Gerard L. – 1985
The specificity of memories has been identified as a factor affecting reality monitoring performance. To examine the reality monitoring model of Johnson and Raye (1981) and to explore the relationship between memory specificity and reality monitoring, the amount of cognitive operations involved in processing information was manipulated for 72…
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Cues, Imagination, Memory

Waddell, Kathryn J.; Rogoff, Barbara – Developmental Psychology, 1987
Study looks at whether spatial memory is automatic by examining the effects of intentionality and attention to contextual organization in spatial memory. The pattern of results demonstrated that reconstruction was enhanced by intentionality or by the goal-relevant activity of attending to contextual spatial relations. (Author/RWB)
Descriptors: Adults, Cognitive Ability, Memory, Recall (Psychology)

Golbeck, Susan L. – International Journal of Behavioral Development, 1985
Examines memory for room-sized spatial arrangement in relation to spatial and classification operations. Sixty first-grade children were given two Piagetian spatial tasks and a two-dimensional point duplication problem. Results of multiple regression showed that Euclidean knowledge (measured by verticality) and age in months predicted memory for…
Descriptors: Classification, Map Skills, Memory, Spatial Ability

Nigro, Georgia N.; Roak, Rebecca M. – American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 1987
Memory automaticity for spatial location was evaluated with 14 mildly retarded adults and 14 nonretarded adults under two instructional conditions: intentional or incidental. Intention to encode spatial location had no effect on recall for either group and retarded and nonretarded subjects did not differ in recall of spatial location. (Author/DB)
Descriptors: Adults, Incidental Learning, Intentional Learning, Memory
Dulaney, Cynthia L.; And Others – American Journal on Mental Retardation, 1996
This study examined recognition memory for items and their location among adults with Down syndrome (n=24), adults with nonspecific mental retardation (n=22), and community volunteers (n=20). No differences in memory for spatial location were found between the two groups with mental retardation, though both groups performed worse than control…
Descriptors: Adults, Cognitive Processes, Downs Syndrome, Memory
Jones, Robert S. P.; Vaughan, Francis L.; Roberts, Mary – American Journal on Mental Retardation, 2002
Comparison for memory for spatial location of 30 persons with and 30 persons without mental retardation found the control group recalled more intentionally learned than incidentally learned locations. The experimental group performed better after incidental learning than after intentional learning and scored as highly as controls on incidental…
Descriptors: Adults, Children, Cognitive Mapping, Incidental Learning

Bebko, James M.; And Others – American Annals of the Deaf, 1992
This study investigated the tendency of deaf children (ages 6-13) not to spontaneously use active memory strategies such as rehearsal. Comparison of 38 deaf and 39 hearing students found that deaf students compensated for less effective rehearsal strategies by capitalizing on unique spatial features of the task. (Author/DB)
Descriptors: Children, Comparative Analysis, Deafness, Elementary Education
Scholnick, Ellin Kofsky; And Others – 1987
Using a map for guiding travel requires: (1) skills in encoding information from a terrain and a map; (2) finding a match between the two; and (3) maintaining the match despite directional shifts from turns on a route. In order to test this analysis, 94 children between the ages of 4 and 6 used maps to locate the route to a goal through a network…
Descriptors: Early Childhood Education, Encoding (Psychology), Geography, Learning Processes

Bain, Sherry K. – Journal of Special Education, 1993
Analysis of Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children (K-ABC) Sequential and Simultaneous Processing scores of 94 children (ages 6-12) with learning disabilities produced factor patterns generally supportive of the traditional K-ABC Mental Processing structure with the exception of Spatial Memory. The sample exhibited relative processing strengths…
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Construct Validity, Diagnostic Tests, Elementary Education

Presson, Clark C. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1987
First and fifth graders participated in this assessment of ways in which differential experience with objects in a spatial array might establish relative landmarks within the array. Results suggest that different levels of experience can establish elements as relative landmarks in spatial memory. (Author/RWB)
Descriptors: Cognitive Ability, Cognitive Development, Cognitive Structures, Comparative Analysis

Foorman, Barbara R.; And Others – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1985
Tests seven- , 10- , and 13-year-olds for developmental differences in processes and strategies involved in geometric matrix solution and the relationship between strategy differences and item complexity. (AS)
Descriptors: Analogy, Developmental Stages, Difficulty Level, Elementary Education
Thomas, Jeanne L. – 1983
Research on adult memory for nonverbal representations has found a significant age-related decrement in performance. To investigate age differences in adult locational versus featural information in visual memory, 90 adult women (29 young women with a mean age of 21; 30 middle aged women with a mean age of 43; and 31 older women with a mean age of…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Aging (Individuals), Cognitive Processes, Eidetic Imagery
Previous Page | Next Page ยป
Pages: 1 | 2