Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 1 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 1 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 5 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 20 |
Descriptor
Spatial Ability | 22 |
Undergraduate Students | 7 |
Correlation | 6 |
Memory | 6 |
Navigation | 5 |
Visual Perception | 4 |
Visualization | 4 |
Cognitive Processes | 3 |
Gender Differences | 3 |
Individual Differences | 3 |
Regression (Statistics) | 3 |
More ▼ |
Source
Author
Newcombe, Nora S. | 5 |
Shipley, Thomas F. | 3 |
Weisberg, Steven M. | 3 |
Durgin, Frank H. | 2 |
Abdul-Malak, Daniel T. | 1 |
Badgio, Daniel | 1 |
Ballator, Nada | 1 |
Blasko, D. G. | 1 |
Bodzin, Alec M. | 1 |
Boyle, L. N. | 1 |
Bradford H. Pillow | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 21 |
Reports - Research | 18 |
Reports - Descriptive | 3 |
Numerical/Quantitative Data | 1 |
Reports - Evaluative | 1 |
Education Level
Higher Education | 12 |
Postsecondary Education | 8 |
Elementary Education | 1 |
Elementary Secondary Education | 1 |
Grade 1 | 1 |
Grade 2 | 1 |
High Schools | 1 |
Kindergarten | 1 |
Middle Schools | 1 |
Secondary Education | 1 |
Audience
Location
Pennsylvania | 22 |
Arizona | 1 |
Australia | 1 |
Indiana | 1 |
Iowa | 1 |
Michigan | 1 |
Nebraska | 1 |
New York | 1 |
North Carolina | 1 |
Texas | 1 |
Virginia | 1 |
More ▼ |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Chun-Hao Chiu; Bradford H. Pillow; The Family Life Project Key Investigators – International Journal of Behavioral Development, 2024
The purpose of this study is to investigate the relations among children's symbolic functioning at 15 months, joint attention at 24 months, expressive communication at 24 and 36 months, and executive functioning at 36 months. With the sample from rural areas in the United States collected by the Family Life Project (N = 1,008), a longitudinal data…
Descriptors: Executive Function, Family Life, Expressive Language, Verbal Communication
Weisberg, Steven M.; Badgio, Daniel; Chatterjee, Anjan – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2018
Knowing where north is provides a navigator with invaluable information for learning and recalling a space, particularly in places with limited navigational cues, like complex indoor environments. Although north is effectively used by orienteers, pilots, and military personnel, very little is known about whether nonexpert populations can or will…
Descriptors: Navigation, Equipment, Tactual Perception, Stimuli
Weckbacher, Lisa Marie; Okamoto, Yukari – Journal of Education and Learning, 2018
There is a consensus that certain cognitive abilities and mathematics performance are related. This link also extends to geometry. Surprisingly, however, little research has examined specific aspects of cognition that may contribute to successful performance in geometry, particularly at the secondary level. In this study, we explored in what ways,…
Descriptors: Cognitive Ability, Mathematics Skills, Cognitive Style, Spatial Ability
Ngo, Chi T.; Weisberg, Steven M.; Newcombe, Nora S.; Olson, Ingrid R. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2016
Although the hippocampus is implicated in both spatial navigation and associative memory, very little is known about whether individual differences in the 2 domains covary. People who prefer to navigate using a hippocampal-dependent place strategy may show better performance on associative memory tasks than those who prefer a caudate-dependent…
Descriptors: Individual Differences, Correlation, Navigation, Association (Psychology)
Holden, Mark P.; Newcombe, Nora S.; Shipley, Thomas F. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2015
Memories for spatial locations often show systematic errors toward the central value of the surrounding region. The Category Adjustment (CA) model suggests that this bias is due to a Bayesian combination of categorical and metric information, which offers an optimal solution under conditions of uncertainty (Huttenlocher, Hedges, & Duncan,…
Descriptors: Spatial Ability, Memory, Models, Task Analysis
Stevens, Christopher A.; Carlson, Richard A. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2016
Many studies have examined how people recall the locations of objects in spatial layouts. However, little is known about how people monitor the accuracy of judgments based on those memories. The goal of the present experiments was to examine the effect of reference frame characteristics on metacognitive accuracy for spatial judgments. Reference…
Descriptors: Spatial Ability, Metacognition, Undergraduate Students, Perspective Taking
Miller, Jonathan F.; Lazarus, Eben M.; Polyn, Sean M.; Kahana, Michael J. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2013
In recalling a list of previously experienced items, participants are known to organize their responses on the basis of the items' semantic and temporal similarities. Here, we examine how spatial information influences the organization of responses in free recall. In Experiment 1, participants studied and subsequently recalled lists of landmarks.…
Descriptors: Memory, Spatial Ability, Recall (Psychology), Responses
Wyble, Brad; Folk, Charles; Potter, Mary C. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2013
Attentional capture is an unintentional shift of visuospatial attention to the location of a distractor that is either highly salient, or relevant to the current task set. The latter situation is referred to as contingent capture, in that the effect is contingent on a match between characteristics of the stimuli and the task-defined…
Descriptors: Stimuli, Classification, Coding, Attention
Weisberg, Steven M.; Schinazi, Victor R.; Newcombe, Nora S.; Shipley, Thomas F.; Epstein, Russell A. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2014
There are marked individual differences in the formation of cognitive maps both in the real world and in virtual environments (VE; e.g., Blajenkova, Motes, & Kozhevnikov, 2005; Chai & Jacobs, 2010; Ishikawa & Montello, 2006; Wen, Ishikawa, & Sato, 2011). These differences, however, are poorly understood and can be difficult to…
Descriptors: Experimental Psychology, Cognitive Mapping, Individual Differences, Simulated Environment
Li, Zhi; Sun, Emily; Strawser, Cassandra J.; Spiegel, Ariana; Klein, Brennan; Durgin, Frank H. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2013
Two experiments are reported concerning the perception of ground extent to discover whether prior reports of anisotropy between frontal extents and extents in depth were consistent across different measures (visual matching and pantomime walking) and test environments (outdoor environments and virtual environments). In Experiment 1 it was found…
Descriptors: Outdoor Education, Depth Perception, Pantomime, Physical Activities
Irons, Jessica L.; Folk, Charles L.; Remington, Roger W. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2012
Although models of visual search have often assumed that attention can only be set for a single feature or property at a time, recent studies have suggested that it may be possible to maintain more than one attentional control setting. The aim of the present study was to investigate whether spatial attention could be guided by multiple attentional…
Descriptors: Attention Control, Spatial Ability, Color, Priming
Harris, Justin; Newcombe, Nora S.; Hirsh-Pasek, Kathy – Mind, Brain, and Education, 2013
The relation of spatial skills to academic success in areas such as math and science has sparked discussion in early education around how spatial thinking skills might be included in early schooling. Planning and evaluating new curricula or interventions requires understanding these skills and having the means to assess them. Prior developmental…
Descriptors: Young Children, Spatial Ability, Thinking Skills, Cognitive Processes
Nardi, Daniele; Newcombe, Nora S.; Shipley, Thomas F. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2011
Studies of spatial representation generally focus on flat environments and visual input. However, the world is not flat, and slopes are part of most natural environments. In a series of 4 experiments, we examined whether humans can use a slope as a source of allocentric, directional information for reorientation. A target was hidden in a corner of…
Descriptors: Spatial Ability, Gender Differences, Orientation, Navigation
The Role of Treatment Fidelity on Outcomes during a Randomized Field Trial of an Autism Intervention
Mandell, David S; Stahmer, Aubyn C; Shin, Sujie; Xie, Ming; Reisinger, Erica; Marcus, Steven C – Autism: The International Journal of Research and Practice, 2013
This randomized field trial comparing Strategies for Teaching based on Autism Research and Structured Teaching enrolled educators in 33 kindergarten-through-second-grade autism support classrooms and 119 students, aged 5-8 years in the School District of Philadelphia. Students were assessed at the beginning and end of the academic year using the…
Descriptors: Comparative Analysis, Autism, Kindergarten, Primary Education
Colaianne, Blake A.; Powell, Matthew G. – Journal of Geoscience Education, 2011
Geology education usually takes place within the context of a broader curriculum, but specific synergies between disciplines have rarely been explored or exploited. Here, we have assessed the spatial visualization skills of undergraduate students in a variety of disciplines to determine which are most compatible with a geology curriculum. Spatial…
Descriptors: Undergraduate Students, Spatial Ability, Gender Differences, Grade Point Average
Previous Page | Next Page ยป
Pages: 1 | 2