NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Showing all 10 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Suegami, Takashi; Laeng, Bruno – Brain and Cognition, 2013
It has been shown that the left and right cerebral hemispheres (LH and RH) respectively process qualitative or "categorical" spatial relations and metric or "coordinate" spatial relations. However, categorical spatial information could be thought as divided into two types: semantically-coded and visuospatially-coded categorical information. We…
Descriptors: Spatial Ability, Semantics, Stimuli, Brain Hemisphere Functions
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Horner, Aidan J.; Henson, Richard N. – Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 2012
Stimulus repetition often leads to facilitated processing, resulting in neural decreases (repetition suppression) and faster RTs (repetition priming). Such repetition-related effects have been attributed to the facilitation of repeated cognitive processes and/or the retrieval of previously encoded stimulus-response (S-R) bindings. Although…
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Evidence, Priming, Classification
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Amorapanth, Prin; Kranjec, Alexander; Bromberger, Bianca; Lehet, Matthew; Widick, Page; Woods, Adam J.; Kimberg, Daniel Y.; Chatterjee, Anjan – Brain and Language, 2012
Schemas are abstract nonverbal representations that parsimoniously depict spatial relations. Despite their ubiquitous use in maps and diagrams, little is known about their neural instantiation. We sought to determine the extent to which schematic representations are neurally distinguished from language on the one hand, and from rich perceptual…
Descriptors: Brain Hemisphere Functions, Patients, Schemata (Cognition), Spatial Ability
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Daniel, Reka; Wagner, Gerd; Koch, Kathrin; Reichenbach, Jurgen R.; Sauer, Heinrich; Schlosser, Ralf G. M. – Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 2011
The formation of new perceptual categories involves learning to extract that information from a wide range of often noisy sensory inputs, which is critical for selecting between a limited number of responses. To identify brain regions involved in visual classification learning under noisy conditions, we developed a task on the basis of the…
Descriptors: Feedback (Response), Classification, Experimental Psychology, Cognitive Processes
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Anthamatten, Peter – Journal of Geography, 2010
Research in the cognition and learning sciences has demonstrated that the human brain contains basic structures whose functions are to perform a variety of specific spatial reasoning tasks and that children are capable of learning basic spatial concepts at an early age. There has been a call from within geography to recognize research on spatial…
Descriptors: Geography Instruction, Geography, Brain Hemisphere Functions, Spatial Ability
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Teuscher, Ursina; Brang, David; Ramachandran, Vilayanur S.; Coulson, Seana – Brain and Cognition, 2010
Some people report that they consistently and involuntarily associate time events, such as months of the year, with specific spatial locations; a condition referred to as time-space synesthesia. The present study investigated the manner in which such synesthetic time-space associations affect visuo-spatial attention via an endogenous cuing…
Descriptors: Cues, Validity, Cognitive Processes, Spatial Ability
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
van Asselen, Marieke; Kessels, Roy P. C.; Frijns, Catharina J. M.; Kappelle, L. Jaap; Neggers, Sebastiaan F. W.; Postma, Albert – Brain and Cognition, 2009
Object-location memory is an important form of spatial memory, comprising different subcomponents that each process specific types of information within memory, i.e. remembering objects, remembering positions and binding these features in memory. In the current study we investigated the neural correlates of binding categorical (relative) or…
Descriptors: Patients, Memory, Neurological Impairments, Spatial Ability
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Courtin, C.; Herve, P. -Y.; Petit, L.; Zago, L.; Vigneau, M.; Beaucousin, V.; Jobard, G.; Mazoyer, B.; Mellet, E.; Tzourio-Mazoyer, N. – Brain and Language, 2010
"Highly iconic" structures in Sign Language enable a narrator to act, switch characters, describe objects, or report actions in four-dimensions. This group of linguistic structures has no real spoken-language equivalent. Topographical descriptions are also achieved in a sign-language specific manner via the use of signing-space and…
Descriptors: Topography, Sign Language, Deafness, Short Term Memory
Matazow, Gail S.; Hynd, George W. – 1992
Children with Attention Deficit Disorder (ADD) often exhibit problems in visual spatial perception, math achievement, and social skills, and it has been postulated that this constellation of behaviors may constitute Right Hemisphere Deficit Syndrome (RHDS). This study examined 21 children with attention deficit disorder with hyperactivity (ADD/H),…
Descriptors: Adolescents, Attention Deficit Disorders, Brain Hemisphere Functions, Children
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Koenig, Oliver; And Others – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1990
Children as young as five years of age responded faster in a categorical task when information was initially presented to the left hemisphere, and faster in a coordinate task when information was initially presented to the right hemisphere. This finding provided evidence for the existence of distinct subsystems that compute categorial and…
Descriptors: Brain Hemisphere Functions, Classification, Cognitive Processes, Coordination