Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 0 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 6 |
Descriptor
Infants | 23 |
Spatial Ability | 23 |
Visual Discrimination | 23 |
Visual Perception | 9 |
Perceptual Development | 6 |
Cognitive Processes | 5 |
Cues | 5 |
Visual Stimuli | 5 |
Age Differences | 4 |
Foreign Countries | 4 |
Infant Behavior | 4 |
More ▼ |
Source
Journal of Experimental Child… | 6 |
Child Development | 4 |
Cognition | 3 |
Developmental Psychology | 3 |
Infancy | 2 |
Cognitive Development | 1 |
Cognitive Psychology | 1 |
Developmental Science | 1 |
Author
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 21 |
Reports - Research | 21 |
Books | 1 |
Collected Works - General | 1 |
Information Analyses | 1 |
Opinion Papers | 1 |
Speeches/Meeting Papers | 1 |
Education Level
Early Childhood Education | 1 |
Audience
Researchers | 1 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Ribordy, Farfalla; Jabes, Adeline; Lavenex, Pamela Banta; Lavenex, Pierre – Cognitive Psychology, 2013
Episodic memories for autobiographical events that happen in unique spatiotemporal contexts are central to defining who we are. Yet, before 2 years of age, children are unable to form or store episodic memories for recall later in life, a phenomenon known as infantile amnesia. Here, we studied the development of allocentric spatial memory, a…
Descriptors: Memory, Toddlers, Rewards, Cues
Hemker, Laura; Granrud, Carl E.; Yonas, Albert; Kavsek, Michael – Infancy, 2010
Two preferential-reaching experiments explored 5- and 7-month-olds' sensitivity to pictorial depth cues. In the first experiment, infants viewed a display in which texture gradients, linear perspective of the surface contours, and relative height in the visual field provided information that two objects were at different distances. Five- and…
Descriptors: Cues, Infants, Pictorial Stimuli, Visual Perception
Lourenco, Stella F.; Huttenlocher, Janellen – Infancy, 2008
There is evidence that, from an early age, humans are sensitive to spatial information such as simple landmarks and the size of objects. This study concerns the ability to represent a particular kind of spatial information, namely, the "geometry" of an enclosed layout--an ability present in older children, adults, and nonhuman animals (e.g.,…
Descriptors: Cues, Infants, Spatial Ability, Geometric Concepts
Bhatt, Ramesh S.; Hayden, Angela; Reed, Andrea; Bertin, Evelin; Joseph, Jane – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2006
Object parts are signaled by concave discontinuities in shape contours. In seven experiments, we examined whether 5- and 6 1/2-month-olds are sensitive to concavities as special aspects of contours. Infants of both ages detected discrepant concave elements amid convex distractors but failed to discriminate convex elements among concave…
Descriptors: Infants, Perception, Child Psychology, Visual Discrimination
Bertenthal, Bennett I.; Longo, Matthew R.; Kenny, Sarah – Child Development, 2007
The perceived spatiotemporal continuity of objects depends on the way they appear and disappear as they move in the spatial layout. This study investigated whether infants' predictive tracking of a briefly occluded object is sensitive to the manner by which the object disappears and reappears. Five-, 7-, and 9-month-old infants were shown a ball…
Descriptors: Kinetics, Infants, Visual Perception, Object Permanence

Quinn, Paul C.; And Others – Developmental Psychology, 1996
Four experiments examined the ability of infants to form categorical representations for the spatial relations "above" and "below." Found that three- and four-month-olds could form categorical representations for above and below when a diamond-shape was presented above or below a horizontal bar but could not do so when a number…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Infants, Spatial Ability, Visual Discrimination

Quinn, Paul C. – Child Development, 1994
Three experiments using the familiarization-novelty preference procedure confirmed the hypothesis that three-month-old infants could form categorical representations of spatial relations above and below. The infants, after being shown a familiarization diagram with a dot appearing in multiple locations below a line, showed a preference for a novel…
Descriptors: Classification, Cognitive Development, Infants, Spatial Ability
Flom, Ross; Bahrick, Lorraine E. – Developmental Psychology, 2007
This research examined the developmental course of infants' ability to perceive affect in bimodal (audiovisual) and unimodal (auditory and visual) displays of a woman speaking. According to the intersensory redundancy hypothesis (L. E. Bahrick, R. Lickliter, & R. Flom, 2004), detection of amodal properties is facilitated in multimodal stimulation…
Descriptors: Stimulation, Social Development, Redundancy, Infants

Colombo, John; And Others – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1984
In three experiments, the effect of additional "contextual" elements on the discrimination of the orientation of linear and curvilinear segments was investigated with four-month-old infants. Results suggest that, regarding certain stimuli and techniques of measurement, surrounding contextual segments can aid the discrimination of linear…
Descriptors: Context Effect, Infant Behavior, Infants, Spatial Ability

Lloyd, S. E.; Freeman, N. H. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1983
A number of hypotheses about infants' delayed search accuracy have been based upon the notion that a location associated with repeated retrieval of an object attains privileged status. However, a test of performance in 12- and 15-month-old infants showed them to be indifferent to location. (Author/RH)
Descriptors: Cognitive Ability, Foreign Countries, Infant Behavior, Infants

Lloyd, S. E.; And Others – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1981
Examined the conditions under which infants employ (1) spatial codes respondent to objects central to their visual field and (2) spatial codes respondent to objects peripheral to their visual field. (DB)
Descriptors: Cognitive Style, Foreign Countries, Infants, Search Strategies

Gilmore, Rick O.; Johnson, Mark H. – Cognition, 1997
Investigated the nature of spatial representations underlying simple visually guided actions with 3- and 7-month-old infants. Saccades in older infants were executed within body-centered spatial coordinates that account for intervening eye movements, whereas younger infants responded according to the target's retinocentric locations without…
Descriptors: Early Experience, Infants, Perceptual Development, Psychomotor Skills

Gergely, Gyorgy; And Others – Cognition, 1995
In a visual habituation experiment, infants watched a circle (the "agent") move toward another circle by jumping over a barrier or jumping without a barrier present, and then watched a circle move straight to another circle. Found that infants were able to identify the agent's spatial goal and to interpret the agent's actions causally in…
Descriptors: Attribution Theory, Foreign Countries, Habituation, Infants
Bertin, Evelin; Bhatt, Ramesh S. – Developmental Science, 2004
Adults readily detect changes in face patterns brought about by the inversion of eyes and mouth when the faces are viewed upright but not when they are viewed upside down. Research suggests that this illusion (the Thatcher illusion) is caused by the interfering effects of face inversion on the processing of second-order relational information…
Descriptors: Spatial Ability, Human Body, Cognitive Processes, Infants

Johnson, Scott P.; Aslin, Richard N. – Cognitive Development, 1996
Two experiments examined the effects of common motion, background texture, and orientation on four-month olds' perception of unity of a partially occluded rod. Results indicated that infants' perception of object unity is not dependent on a single visual cue but on a variety of cues including motion, interposition, depth cues, background texture,…
Descriptors: Depth Perception, Infants, Motion, Object Permanence
Previous Page | Next Page ยป
Pages: 1 | 2