Descriptor
Depth Perception | 10 |
Perceptual Development | 10 |
Spatial Ability | 10 |
Age Differences | 5 |
Visual Perception | 5 |
Cognitive Development | 4 |
Infants | 4 |
Freehand Drawing | 3 |
Art Education | 2 |
Art Expression | 2 |
Childrens Art | 2 |
More ▼ |
Source
Cognitive Development | 2 |
Developmental Psychology | 2 |
Journal of Experimental Child… | 2 |
Visual Arts Research | 2 |
Practical Pointers | 1 |
Author
Arterberry, Martha | 1 |
Aslin, Richard N. | 1 |
Batten, Annabel | 1 |
Bin, I. | 1 |
Bremner, J. Gavin | 1 |
Chandler, Kacey | 1 |
Crawford, Connie M. | 1 |
Johnson, Scott P. | 1 |
Kaufmann, Ruth | 1 |
Milbrath, Constance | 1 |
Park, Eundeok | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 8 |
Reports - Research | 7 |
Reports - Descriptive | 2 |
Collected Works - Serials | 1 |
Guides - Classroom - Teacher | 1 |
Reference Materials -… | 1 |
Education Level
Audience
Practitioners | 1 |
Researchers | 1 |
Teachers | 1 |
Location
United Kingdom (London) | 1 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating

Arterberry, Martha; And Others – Developmental Psychology, 1989
Results indicate that seven-month-old infants are sensitive to the depth cues of linear perspective and texture gradients. Self-produced locomotor experience is not necessary for the development of sensitivity to static-monocular depth information. (RJC)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Depth Perception, Infants, Perceptual Development

Johnson, Scott P.; Aslin, Richard N. – Cognitive Development, 1996
Two experiments examined the effects of common motion, background texture, and orientation on four-month olds' perception of unity of a partially occluded rod. Results indicated that infants' perception of object unity is not dependent on a single visual cue but on a variety of cues including motion, interposition, depth cues, background texture,…
Descriptors: Depth Perception, Infants, Motion, Object Permanence

Pickens, Jeffrey – Developmental Psychology, 1994
Sixty-four infants viewed side-by-side videotapes of toy trains (in four visual conditions) and listened to sounds at increasing or decreasing amplitude designed to match one of the videos. Results suggested that five-month olds were sensitive to auditory-visual distance relations and that change in size was an important visual depth cue. (MDM)
Descriptors: Auditory Discrimination, Cues, Depth Perception, Distance

Kaufmann, Ruth; And Others – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1981
An Ames static trapezoidal window was used to test infants' responsiveness to pictorial depth. Sensitivity to the pictorial information for depth that is present in the trapezoidal window appears to develop after the age of 22 weeks. (Author/DB)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Depth Perception, Infant Behavior, Infants

Rosser, Rosemary A.; Chandler, Kacey – Cognitive Development, 1995
Examined how children's and adults' initial conceptions of objects and space influence predictions about the physical world, but lead the naive person to misconstrue a dynamic event. Found that participants proficiently anticipated where an oscillating screen would contact a hidden object, but underestimated the distance until contact.…
Descriptors: Adults, Age Differences, Cognitive Development, Depth Perception

Bremner, J. Gavin; Batten, Annabel – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1991
In this study of sensitivity to viewpoint, children between the ages of 6 and 14 years were asked to draw an L-shaped array of 3 cubes from 1 of 3 views. At every age, children showed sensitivity to their view in the sense that there were consistent differences between the drawings produced in the three viewing conditions. (SH)
Descriptors: Adolescents, Age Differences, Children, Cognitive Development
Sorrell, Howard M. – Practical Pointers, 1978
A circuit approach and station techniques are used to depict perceptual motor games for handicapped and nonhandicapped children. Twenty activities are described in terms of objectives, materials, and procedures, and their focus on visual tracking, visual discrimination and copying of forms, spatial body perception, fine motor coordination, tactile…
Descriptors: Class Activities, Depth Perception, Disabilities, Elementary Secondary Education
Crawford, Connie M. – 1995
In 1988, 59 Kindergarten students were studied to determine the effect of "shadowing" on perceptual-motor learning. Shadowing is a method whereby the use of one's shadow provides visual feedback. The method developed from observing children's natural curiosity in creating shadows. Illuminated by sunlight outdoors or overhead projectors…
Descriptors: Body Image, Depth Perception, Early Childhood Education, Eye Hand Coordination

Milbrath, Constance; Siegel, Bryna – Visual Arts Research, 1996
Traces the development of artistic ability in a six-year-old boy with autism and mild mental retardation. Addresses questions concerning autism and artistic development including whether intellectual realism is a necessary precursor to visual realism and whether a two-dimensional photographic memory is used as opposed to three-dimensional…
Descriptors: Art Education, Art Expression, Art Therapy, Autism

Park, Eundeok; Bin, I. – Visual Arts Research, 1995
Analyzes the research strategies, stimuli, subjects, statistical strategies, and relative variables in 34 empirical studies on children's representation of three-dimensional objects. The studies fell into three categories: children's representation of spatial relationships within an object, between two objects, and studies that included both. (MJP)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Art Activities, Art Education, Art Expression