Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 0 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 23 |
Descriptor
Speech | 25 |
Language Processing | 11 |
Phonology | 6 |
Semantics | 6 |
Word Recognition | 6 |
Oral Language | 5 |
Syllables | 5 |
Acoustics | 4 |
Cues | 4 |
Experiments | 4 |
Phonetics | 4 |
More ▼ |
Source
Journal of Memory and Language | 25 |
Author
Mitterer, Holger | 2 |
Adam, Robert | 1 |
Alario, F.-Xavier | 1 |
Barnes, Lisa | 1 |
Bastiaanse, Roelien | 1 |
Bion, Ricardo A. H. | 1 |
Bos, Laura S. | 1 |
Bresnan, Joan | 1 |
Budd, Mary-Jane | 1 |
Burki, Audrey | 1 |
Cho, Taehong | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 25 |
Reports - Research | 21 |
Reports - Descriptive | 2 |
Opinion Papers | 1 |
Education Level
Audience
Location
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Kuperman, Victor; Bresnan, Joan – Journal of Memory and Language, 2012
In a series of seven studies, this paper examines acoustic characteristics of the spontaneous speech production of the English dative alternation ("gave the book to the boy/ the boy the book") as a function of the probability of the choice between alternating constructions. Probabilistic effects on the acoustic duration were observed in the…
Descriptors: Speech Communication, Speech, Acoustics, Probability
Konopka, Agnieszka E. – Journal of Memory and Language, 2012
The scope of linguistic planning, i.e., the amount of linguistic information that speakers prepare in advance for an utterance they are about to produce, is highly variable. Distinguishing between possible sources of this variability provides a way to discriminate between production accounts that assume structurally incremental and lexically…
Descriptors: Language Processing, Planning, Speech, Sentence Structure
Dumay, Nicolas; Content, Alain – Journal of Memory and Language, 2012
Two auditory priming experiments tested whether the effect of final phonological overlap relies on syllabic representations. Amount of shared phonemic information and syllabic status of the overlap between nonword primes and targets were varied orthogonally. In the related conditions, CV.CCVC items shared the last syllable (e.g., vi.klyd-p[image…
Descriptors: Priming, Syllables, Phonemes, Auditory Perception
Mitterer, Holger; Kim, Sahyang; Cho, Taehong – Journal of Memory and Language, 2013
In connected speech, phonological assimilation to neighboring words can lead to pronunciation variants (e.g., "garden bench" [arrow right] "garde'm' bench"). A large body of literature suggests that listeners use the phonetic context to reconstruct the intended word for assimilation types that often lead to incomplete assimilations (e.g., a…
Descriptors: Korean, Pronunciation, Phonology, Phonetics
Kim, Dahee; Stephens, Joseph D. W.; Pitt, Mark A. – Journal of Memory and Language, 2012
Four experiments examined listeners' segmentation of ambiguous schwa-initial sequences (e.g., "a long" vs. "along") in casual speech, where acoustic cues can be unclear, possibly increasing reliance on contextual information to resolve the ambiguity. In Experiment 1, acoustic analyses of talkers' productions showed that the one-word and two-word…
Descriptors: Cues, Speech, Figurative Language, Acoustics
Gahl, Susanne; Yao, Yao; Johnson, Keith – Journal of Memory and Language, 2012
Frequent or contextually predictable words are often phonetically reduced, i.e. shortened and produced with articulatory undershoot. Explanations for phonetic reduction of predictable forms tend to take one of two approaches: Intelligibility-based accounts hold that talkers maximize intelligibility of words that might otherwise be difficult to…
Descriptors: Speech, Phonetics, Language Acquisition, Vowels
Mol, Lisette; Krahmer, Emiel; Maes, Alfons; Swerts, Marc – Journal of Memory and Language, 2012
Interlocutors sometimes repeat each other's co-speech hand gestures. In three experiments, we investigate to what extent the copying of such gestures' form is tied to their meaning in the linguistic context, as well as to interlocutors' representations of this meaning at the conceptual level. We found that gestures were repeated only if they could…
Descriptors: Evidence, Nonverbal Communication, Speech, Motor Reactions
A Written Word Is Worth a Thousand Spoken Words: The Influence of Spelling on Spoken-Word Production
Burki, Audrey; Spinelli, Elsa; Gaskell, M. Gareth – Journal of Memory and Language, 2012
The present study investigated the role of spelling in phonological variant processing. Participants learned the auditory forms of potential reduced variants of novel French words (e.g., /plur/) and their associations with pictures of novel objects over 4 days. After the fourth day of training, the spelling of each novel word was presented once.…
Descriptors: Spelling, Speech, Phonology, Language Processing
Newman, Rochelle S.; Sawusch, James R.; Wunnenberg, Tyler – Journal of Memory and Language, 2011
Fluent speech does not contain obvious breaks to word boundaries, yet there are a number of cues that listeners can use to help them segment the speech stream. Most of these cues have been investigated in isolation from one another. In previous work, Norris, McQueen, Cutler, and Butterfield (1997) suggested that listeners use a Possible Word…
Descriptors: Cues, Speech, Acoustics, Syllables
Tydgat, Ilse; Stevens, Michael; Hartsuiker, Robert J.; Pickering, Martin J. – Journal of Memory and Language, 2011
This study investigated whether speakers strategically decide where to interrupt their speech once they need to stop. We conducted four naming experiments in which pictures of colored shapes occasionally changed in color or shape. Participants then merely had to stop (Experiment 1); or they had to stop and resume speech (Experiments 2-4). They…
Descriptors: Speech, Decision Making, Sentence Structure, Nouns
Runnqvist, Elin; Strijkers, Kristof; Alario, F.-Xavier; Costa, Albert – Journal of Memory and Language, 2012
Several studies have shown that concepts spread activation to words of both of a bilingual's languages. Therefore, a central issue that needs to be clarified is how a bilingual manages to restrict his speech production to a single language. One influential proposal is that when speaking in one language, the other language is inhibited. An…
Descriptors: Speech, Semantics, Interference (Language), Spanish
Goswami, Usha; Mead, Natasha; Fosker, Tim; Huss, Martina; Barnes, Lisa; Leong, Victoria – Journal of Memory and Language, 2013
Prosodic patterning is a key structural element of spoken language. However, the potential role of prosodic awareness in the phonological difficulties that characterise children with developmental dyslexia has been little studied. Here we report the first longitudinal study of sensitivity to syllable stress in children with dyslexia, enabling the…
Descriptors: Dyslexia, Speech, Syllables, Phonological Awareness
Recognition of Signed and Spoken Language: Different Sensory Inputs, the Same Segmentation Procedure
Orfanidou, Eleni; Adam, Robert; Morgan, Gary; McQueen, James M. – Journal of Memory and Language, 2010
Signed languages are articulated through simultaneous upper-body movements and are seen; spoken languages are articulated through sequential vocal-tract movements and are heard. But word recognition in both language modalities entails segmentation of a continuous input into discrete lexical units. According to the Possible Word Constraint (PWC),…
Descriptors: Speech, Sign Language, Oral Language, Deafness
Kolan, Limor; Leikin, Mark; Zwitserlood, Pienie – Journal of Memory and Language, 2011
This study investigated the nature of the retrieval architecture of Semitic morphemic entities in word production in Hebrew, a language with a non-concatenated morphology. By taking advantage of the potential for dissociation of form and meaning in Hebrew, we explored the relative contribution of word-form and semantics to morphological…
Descriptors: Evidence, Semitic Languages, Architecture, Speech
Dragoy, Olga; Stowe, Laurie A.; Bos, Laura S.; Bastiaanse, Roelien – Journal of Memory and Language, 2012
Time reference in Indo-European languages is marked on the verb. With tensed verb forms, the speaker can refer to the past (wrote, has written), present (writes, is writing) or future (will write). Reference to the past through verb morphology has been shown to be particularly vulnerable in agrammatic aphasia and both agrammatic and…
Descriptors: Reaction Time, Verbs, Language Processing, Indo European Languages
Previous Page | Next Page ยป
Pages: 1 | 2