NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Audience
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
MacArthur Communicative…1
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Showing 1 to 15 of 28 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Gibson, Dominic J.; Gunderson, Elizabeth A.; Spaepen, Elizabet; Levine, Susan C.; Goldin-Meadow, Susan – Developmental Science, 2019
When asked to explain their solutions to a problem, children often gesture and, at times, these gestures convey information that is different from the information conveyed in speech. Children who produce these gesture-speech "mismatches" on a particular task have been found to profit from instruction on that task. We have recently found…
Descriptors: Numbers, Nonverbal Communication, Teaching Methods, Speech Communication
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Demir-Lira, Özlem Ece; Asaridou, Salomi S.; Raja Beharelle, Anjali; Holt, Anna E.; Goldin-Meadow, Susan; Small, Steven L. – Developmental Science, 2018
Gesture is an integral part of children's communicative repertoire. However, little is known about the neurobiology of speech and gesture integration in the developing brain. We investigated how 8- to 10-year-old children processed gesture that was essential to understanding a set of narratives. We asked whether the functional neuroanatomy of…
Descriptors: Nonverbal Communication, Neurology, Biology, Brain Hemisphere Functions
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Frausel, Rebecca R.; Richland, Lindsey E.; Levine, Susan C.; Goldin-Meadow, Susan – Developmental Psychology, 2021
Personal narrative is decontextualized talk where individuals recount stories of personal experience about past or future events. As an everyday discursive speech type, narrative potentially invites parents and children to explicitly link together, generalize from, and make inferences about representations--that is, to engage in higher-order…
Descriptors: Parent Child Relationship, Thinking Skills, Family Environment, Personal Narratives
Frausel, Rebecca R.; Richland, Lindsey E.; Levine, Susan C.; Goldin-Meadow, Susan – Grantee Submission, 2021
Personal narrative is decontextualized talk where individuals recount stories of personal experiences about past or future events. As an everyday discursive speech type, narrative potentially invites parents and children to explicitly link together, generalize from, and make inferences about representations--i.e., to engage in higher-order…
Descriptors: Parent Child Relationship, Thinking Skills, Family Environment, Personal Narratives
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
PDF on ERIC Download full text
Brookshire, Geoffrey; Lu, Jenny; Nusbaum, Howard; Goldin-Meadow, Susan; Casasanto, Daniel – Grantee Submission, 2017
Despite immense variability across languages, people can learn to understand any human language, spoken or signed. What neural mechanisms allow people to comprehend language across sensory modalities? When people listen to speech, electrophysiological oscillations in auditory cortex entrain to slow (<8 Hz) fluctuations in the acoustic envelope.…
Descriptors: Brain Hemisphere Functions, Visual Perception, Auditory Perception, Learning Modalities
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Özçaliskan, Seyda; Lucero, Ché; Goldin-Meadow, Susan – Cognitive Science, 2018
Sighted speakers of different languages vary systematically in how they package and order components of a motion event in speech. These differences influence how semantic elements are organized in gesture, but only when those gestures are produced with speech (co-speech gesture), not without speech (silent gesture). We ask whether the…
Descriptors: Blindness, Adults, Native Speakers, English
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Demir, Özlem Ece; Levine, Susan C.; Goldin-Meadow, Susan – Journal of Child Language, 2015
Speakers of all ages spontaneously gesture as they talk. These gestures predict children's milestones in vocabulary and sentence structure. We ask whether gesture serves a similar role in the development of narrative skill. Children were asked to retell a story conveyed in a wordless cartoon at age five and then again at six, seven, and eight.…
Descriptors: Nonverbal Communication, Speech Communication, Predictor Variables, Vocabulary Development
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
LeBarton, Eve Sauer; Goldin-Meadow, Susan; Raudenbush, Stephen – Journal of Cognition and Development, 2015
Differences in vocabulary that children bring with them to school can be traced back to the gestures they produced at the age of 1;2, which, in turn, can be traced back to the gestures their parents produced at the same age (Rowe & Goldin-Meadow, 2009a). We ask here whether child gesture can be experimentally increased and, if so, whether the…
Descriptors: Nonverbal Communication, Vocabulary Development, Intervention, Oral Language
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Shneidman, Laura A.; Arroyo, Michelle E.; Levine, Susan C.; Goldin-Meadow, Susan – Journal of Child Language, 2013
The talk children hear from their primary caregivers predicts the size of their vocabularies. But children who spend time with multiple individuals also hear talk that others direct to them, as well as talk not directed to them at all. We investigated the effect of linguistic input on vocabulary acquisition in children who routinely spent time…
Descriptors: Linguistic Input, Vocabulary Development, Caregiver Child Relationship, Parent Child Relationship
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Cartmill, Erica A.; Hunsicker, Dea; Goldin-Meadow, Susan – Developmental Psychology, 2014
Nouns form the first building blocks of children's language but are not consistently modified by other words until around 2.5 years of age. Before then, children often combine their nouns with gestures that indicate the object labeled by the noun, for example, pointing at a bottle while saying "bottle." These gestures are typically…
Descriptors: Child Language, Nouns, Nonverbal Communication, Form Classes (Languages)
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Gentner, Dedre; Ozyurek, Asli; Gurcanli, Ozge; Goldin-Meadow, Susan – Cognition, 2013
Does spatial language influence how people think about space? To address this question, we observed children who did not know a conventional language, and tested their performance on nonlinguistic spatial tasks. We studied deaf children living in Istanbul whose hearing losses prevented them from acquiring speech and whose hearing parents had not…
Descriptors: Spatial Ability, Linguistic Input, Deafness, Children
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Ozcaliskan, Seyda; Levine, Susan C.; Goldin-Meadow, Susan – Journal of Child Language, 2013
Children with pre/perinatal unilateral brain lesions (PL) show remarkable plasticity for language development. Is this plasticity characterized by the same developmental trajectory that characterizes typically developing (TD) children, with gesture leading the way into speech? We explored this question, comparing eleven children with PL -- matched…
Descriptors: Brain, Injuries, Prenatal Influences, Perinatal Influences
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Cook, Susan Wagner; Yip, Terina Kuangyi; Goldin-Meadow, Susan – Language and Cognitive Processes, 2012
Gesturing is ubiquitous in communication and serves an important function for listeners, who are able to glean meaningful information from the gestures they see. But gesturing also functions for speakers, whose own gestures reduce demands on their working memory. Here we ask whether gesture's beneficial effects on working memory stem from its…
Descriptors: Short Term Memory, Nonverbal Communication, Speech Communication, Mathematics
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Sauter, Megan; Uttal, David H.; Alman, Amanda Schaal; Goldin-Meadow, Susan; Levine, Susan C. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2012
This article examines two issues: the role of gesture in the communication of spatial information and the relation between communication and mental representation. Children (8-10 years) and adults walked through a space to learn the locations of six hidden toy animals and then explained the space to another person. In Study 1, older children and…
Descriptors: Animals, Nonverbal Communication, Spatial Ability, Children
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Cook, Susan Wagner; Yip, Terina KuangYi; Goldin-Meadow, Susan – Journal of Memory and Language, 2010
When people are asked to perform actions, they remember those actions better than if they are asked to talk about the same actions. But when people talk, they often gesture with their hands, thus adding an action component to talking. The question we asked in this study was whether producing gesture along with speech makes the information encoded…
Descriptors: Nonverbal Communication, Interpersonal Communication, Speech Communication, Recall (Psychology)
Previous Page | Next Page »
Pages: 1  |  2