NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Showing all 6 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Alberto Parola; Dize Hilviu; Sara Vivaldo; Andrea Marini; Diego Di Lisi; Patrizia Consolino; Francesca Marina Bosco – Journal of Child Language, 2023
Cochlear Implants (CIs) enhance linguistic skills in deaf or hard of hearing children (D/HH). However, the benefits of CIs have not been sufficiently studied, especially with regard to communicative-pragmatics, i.e., the ability to communicate appropriately in a specific context using different expressive means, such as language and…
Descriptors: Assistive Technology, Deafness, Students with Disabilities, Pragmatics
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
PDF on ERIC Download full text
Hill, Amanda – Journal of Communication Pedagogy, 2021
Today's students often speak through mediated technologies. Thus, understanding how nonverbal cues impact meaning-making is key to understanding effective communication across mediums. This case study explores a group project where students created audio podcasts to teach others about a specific aspect of communication studies while considering…
Descriptors: Information Dissemination, Audio Equipment, Communications, Acoustics
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Iglesias Fernández, Emilia; Russo, Mariachiara – Interpreter and Translator Trainer, 2020
The complex nature of remote interpreting (RI) demands a multidisciplinary approach. The present article focuses on telephone interpreting (TI) in the light of the most relevant disciplines to suggest a coherent theoretical and methodologic framework. This approach will contribute to analysing TI components and mechanics, both for research…
Descriptors: Translation, Language Processing, Interdisciplinary Approach, Intonation
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Waxer, Matthew; Morton, J. Bruce – Child Development, 2011
Six-year-old children can judge a speaker's feelings either from content or paralanguage but have difficulty switching the basis of their judgments when these cues conflict. This inflexibility may relate to a lexical bias in 6-year-olds' judgments. Two experiments tested this claim. In Experiment 1, 6-year-olds (n = 40) were as inflexible when…
Descriptors: Cues, Paralinguistics, Child Development, Young Children
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Yow, W. Quin; Markman, Ellen M. – Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 2011
Preschoolers tend to rely on what speakers say rather than how they sound when interpreting a speaker's emotion while adults rely instead on tone of voice. However, children who have a greater need to attend to speakers' communicative requirements, such as bilingual children, may be more adept in using paralinguistic cues (e.g. tone of voice) when…
Descriptors: Cues, Paralinguistics, Monolingualism, Bilingualism
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Gauster, Andrea; Yunusova, Yana; Zajac, David – Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 2010
The purpose of this study was to assess the effect of speaking rate variation on aerodynamic and acoustic measures of velopharyngeal (VP) function. Twenty-seven healthy adult speakers (14 males, 13 females) participated in the study. The modified pressure-flow method was used to collect aerodynamic data of /m/ and /p/ segments in the word…
Descriptors: Sentences, Speech Communication, Speech Acts, Females