Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 3 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 3 |
Descriptor
Associative Learning | 7 |
College Students | 7 |
Stimuli | 7 |
Accuracy | 2 |
Classification | 2 |
Concept Formation | 2 |
Discrimination Learning | 2 |
Higher Education | 2 |
Learning Processes | 2 |
Responses | 2 |
Abstract Reasoning | 1 |
More ▼ |
Source
Journal of Experimental… | 3 |
Alberta Journal of… | 1 |
Cognitive Psychology | 1 |
J Consult Clin Psychol | 1 |
J Exp Psychol | 1 |
Author
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 5 |
Reports - Research | 5 |
Education Level
Higher Education | 3 |
Postsecondary Education | 3 |
Audience
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
George, David N.; Oltean, Bianca P. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2020
Learning to categorize perceptually similar stimuli can result in people becoming more sensitive to differences along perceptual dimensions that are relevant to category membership and/or less sensitive to equivalent differences along irrelevant perceptual dimensions. These effects of acquired distinctiveness and acquired equivalence may be caused…
Descriptors: Foreign Countries, College Students, Associative Learning, Learning Processes
Ashby, F. Gregory; Vucovich, Lauren E. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2016
Feedback is highly contingent on behavior if it eventually becomes easy to predict, and weakly contingent on behavior if it remains difficult or impossible to predict even after learning is complete. Many studies have demonstrated that humans and nonhuman animals are highly sensitive to feedback contingency, but no known studies have examined how…
Descriptors: Feedback (Response), Classification, Learning Processes, Associative Learning
Francis, Wendy S.; Strobach, E. Natalia; Penalver, Renee M.; Martínez, Michelle; Gurrola, Bianca V.; Soltero, Amaris – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2019
Three source-memory experiments were conducted with Spanish-English bilinguals and monolingual English speakers matched on age, education, nonverbal cognitive ability and socioeconomic status. Bilingual language proficiency and dominance were assessed using standardized objective measures. In Experiment 1, source was manipulated visuo-spatially,…
Descriptors: Associative Learning, Short Term Memory, Context Effect, Concept Formation
Restle, Frank; Brown, Eric R. – J Exp Psychol, 1970
Descriptors: Associative Learning, College Students, Serial Learning, Stimuli

Hayduk, Allan W.; Osborne, John W. – Alberta Journal of Educational Research, 1981
The fact that a significant amount of release from proactive interference was obtained with subjects in this study by shifting between differentially arousing categories of words suggested that rated word arousal is an encoding dimension in short-term memory. (CM)
Descriptors: Associative Learning, College Students, Higher Education, Memory
Galbraith, Gary G.; Mosher, Donald L. – J Consult Clin Psychol, 1970
Free associations to double-entendre sexual and asexual words were elicited from college males under conditions of sexual stimulation and no stimulation (control). Results indicated significant interaction between stimulation and guilt and sexuality-asexuality of words might be factor in recall errors of high-and low-sex-guilt Ss. (Author)
Descriptors: Associative Learning, College Students, Males, Responses

Busemeyer, Jerome; And Others – Cognitive Psychology, 1997
A new paradigm is presented for investigating how intervening concepts are learned. Results of four experiments involving 85 college students provide converging evidence for the acquisition of intervening concepts. A simple associative learning mechanism is proposed to account for the results. The new paradigm uses a stimulus-response-feedback…
Descriptors: Abstract Reasoning, Associative Learning, College Students, Concept Formation