Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 1 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 8 |
Descriptor
Associative Learning | 8 |
Stimuli | 8 |
Task Analysis | 8 |
Animals | 4 |
Brain Hemisphere Functions | 3 |
Cues | 3 |
Classification | 2 |
Correlation | 2 |
Feedback (Response) | 2 |
Foreign Countries | 2 |
Learning Processes | 2 |
More ▼ |
Author
Abel, Ted | 1 |
Ashby, F. Gregory | 1 |
Barrett, Louise C. | 1 |
Beran, Michael J. | 1 |
Braem, Senne | 1 |
Chan, Betty | 1 |
Eichenbaum, Howard | 1 |
Evans, Theodore A. | 1 |
Flemming, Timothy M. | 1 |
Hannenhalli, Sridhar | 1 |
Harris, Emily H. | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 8 |
Reports - Research | 8 |
Education Level
Higher Education | 2 |
Postsecondary Education | 1 |
Audience
Location
Australia | 1 |
California (Santa Barbara) | 1 |
Spain | 1 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Ashby, F. Gregory; Vucovich, Lauren E. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2016
Feedback is highly contingent on behavior if it eventually becomes easy to predict, and weakly contingent on behavior if it remains difficult or impossible to predict even after learning is complete. Many studies have demonstrated that humans and nonhuman animals are highly sensitive to feedback contingency, but no known studies have examined how…
Descriptors: Feedback (Response), Classification, Learning Processes, Associative Learning
Ortells, Juan J.; Mari-Beffa, Paloma; Plaza-Ayllon, Vanesa – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2013
Participants performed a 2-choice categorization task on visible word targets that were preceded by novel (unpracticed) prime words. The prime words were presented for 33 ms and followed either immediately (Experiments 1-3) or after a variable delay (Experiments 1 and 4) by a pattern mask. Both subjective and objective measures of prime visibility…
Descriptors: Semantics, Priming, Decision Making, Classification
Braem, Senne; Verguts, Tom; Notebaert, Wim – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2011
Cognitive control is responsible for adapting information processing in order to carry out tasks more efficiently. Contrasting global versus local control accounts, it has recently been proposed that control operates in an associative fashion, that is, by binding stimulus-response associations after detection of conflict (Verguts & Notebaert,…
Descriptors: Conflict, Associative Learning, Cognitive Processes, Prediction
Tort, Adriano B. L.; Komorowski, Robert; Kopell, Nancy; Eichenbaum, Howard – Learning & Memory, 2011
The association of specific events with the context in which they occur is a fundamental feature of episodic memory. However, the underlying network mechanisms generating what-where associations are poorly understood. Recently we reported that some hippocampal principal neurons develop representations of specific events occurring in particular…
Descriptors: Animals, Brain Hemisphere Functions, Context Effect, Correlation
Barrett, Louise C.; Livesey, Evan J. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2010
Perruchet, Cleeremans, and Destrebecqz (2006) reported a striking dissociation between trends in the conscious expectancy of an event and the speed of a response that is cued by that event. They argued that this indicates the operation of independent processes in human associative learning. However, there remains a strong possibility that this…
Descriptors: Stimuli, Reaction Time, Associative Learning, Learning Processes
Beran, Michael J.; Klein, Emily D.; Evans, Theodore A.; Chan, Betty; Flemming, Timothy M.; Harris, Emily H.; Washburn, David A.; Rumbaugh, Duane M. – Psychological Record, 2008
Learning styles in capuchin monkeys were assessed with a computerized reversal-learning task called the mediational paradigm. First, monkeys were trained to respond with 90% accuracy on a two-choice discrimination (A+B-). Then the authors examined differences in performance on three different types of reversal trials (A-B+, A-C+, B+C-), each of…
Descriptors: Cues, Teaching Methods, Prediction, Animals
Miranda, Maria Isabel; Quirarte, Gina L.; Rodriguez-Garcia, Gabriela; McGaugh, James L.; Roozendaal, Benno – Learning & Memory, 2008
It is well established that glucocorticoid hormones strengthen the consolidation of hippocampus-dependent spatial and contextual memory. The present experiments investigated glucocorticoid effects on the long-term formation of conditioned taste aversion (CTA), an associative learning task that does not depend critically on hippocampal function.…
Descriptors: Stimuli, Associative Learning, Memory, Brain Hemisphere Functions
Isiegas, Carolina; Stein, Joel; Hellman, Kevin; Hannenhalli, Sridhar; Abel, Ted; Keeley, Michael B.; Wood, Marcelo A. – Learning & Memory, 2006
Classical fear conditioning requires the recognition of conditioned stimuli (CS) and the association of the CS with an aversive stimulus. We used Affymetrix oligonucleotide microarrays to characterize changes in gene expression compared to naive mice in both the amygdala and the hippocampus 30 min after classical fear conditioning and 30 min after…
Descriptors: Fear, Genetics, Stimuli, Animals