Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 1 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 3 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 8 |
Descriptor
Source
Journal of Experimental… | 3 |
Journal of the Experimental… | 3 |
Cognitive Science | 1 |
Journal of Behavioral… | 1 |
Teaching of Psychology | 1 |
Author
Dougher, Michael J. | 2 |
Derenne, Adam | 1 |
Doan, Charles A. | 1 |
Dreisbach, Gesine | 1 |
Dry, Matthew J. | 1 |
Fink, Brandi | 1 |
Freedberg, Michael | 1 |
Greenway, David E. | 1 |
Hamilton, Derek | 1 |
Harrington, Jennifer | 1 |
Hazeltine, Eliot | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 9 |
Reports - Research | 8 |
Reports - Descriptive | 1 |
Education Level
Higher Education | 9 |
Postsecondary Education | 9 |
Audience
Location
Australia | 1 |
Germany | 1 |
Iowa | 1 |
New Mexico | 1 |
Ohio | 1 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Vigo, Ronaldo; Doan, Charles A.; Zhao, Li – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2022
The quest for determining the degree of learning difficulty associated with different types of categories has been instrumental in our understanding of human categorization behavior and, more broadly, human generalization. For instance, we now know that the topological nature of the dimensions (e.g., whether these are integral or separable) that…
Descriptors: Discrimination Learning, Classification, Learning Processes, Difficulty Level
Hefer, Carmen; Dreisbach, Gesine – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2020
There is much evidence showing that the prospect of performance-contingent reward increases the usage of cuing information and cognitive stability. In a recent study, we showed that participants under reward conditions even continued using cues even when they were no longer predictive of the required response rule, even at the expense of higher…
Descriptors: Rewards, Contingency Management, Cues, Reaction Time
Meindl, James N.; Ivy, Jonathan W.; Miller, Neal; Neef, Nancy A.; Williamson, Robert L. – Journal of Behavioral Education, 2013
Fluency-based strategies such as Say All Fast a Minute Each Day Shuffled (SAFMEDS) effectively promote fluent responding (i.e., high rate and accuracy). It is possible, however, that the stimulus control developed through these activities inhibits stimulus generalization. We investigated this concern in a two-part study with college students.…
Descriptors: Generalization, Instructional Materials, College Students, Verbal Stimuli
Freedberg, Michael; Schacherer, Jonathan; Hazeltine, Eliot – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2016
Reward has been shown to change behavior as a result of incentive learning (by motivating the individual to increase their effort) and instrumental learning (by increasing the frequency of a particular behavior). However, Palminteri et al. (2011) demonstrated that reward can also improve the incidental learning of a motor skill even when…
Descriptors: Incidental Learning, Associative Learning, Rewards, Incentives
Navarro, Daniel J.; Dry, Matthew J.; Lee, Michael D. – Cognitive Science, 2012
Inductive generalization, where people go beyond the data provided, is a basic cognitive capability, and it underpins theoretical accounts of learning, categorization, and decision making. To complete the inductive leap needed for generalization, people must make a key "sampling" assumption about how the available data were generated.…
Descriptors: Logical Thinking, Generalization, Sampling, Learning
Derenne, Adam – Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 2010
A shift in generalization gradients away from S+ and towards stimuli on the opposite end of the stimulus dimension from S- is a well established phenomenon in the laboratory, occurring with humans and nonhumans and with a wide range of stimuli. The phenomenon of gradient shifts has also been observed to have an analogous relationship to a variety…
Descriptors: Stimulus Generalization, Discrimination Learning, Shift Studies, Visual Stimuli
Perkins, David R.; Dougher, Michael J.; Greenway, David E. – Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 2007
This study investigated conditions leading to contextual control by stimulus topography over transfer of functions. Three 4-member stimulus equivalence classes, each consisting of four (A, B, C, D) topographically distinct visual stimuli, were established for 5 college students. Across classes, designated A stimuli were open-ended linear figures,…
Descriptors: Visual Stimuli, Behavior, Reinforcement, Association (Psychology)
Dougher, Michael J.; Hamilton, Derek; Fink, Brandi; Harrington, Jennifer – Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 2007
In three experiments, match-to-sample procedures were used with undergraduates to establish arbitrary relational functions for three abstract visual stimuli. In the presence of samples A, B, and C, participants were trained to select the smallest, middle, and largest member, respectively, of a series of three-comparison arrays. In Experiment 1,…
Descriptors: Stimulus Generalization, Association (Psychology), Visual Stimuli, Classical Conditioning
Keith, Kenneth D. – Teaching of Psychology, 2002
Stimulus discrimination is a standard subject in undergraduate courses presenting basic principles of learning, and a particularly interesting aspect of discrimination is the peak shift phenomenon. Peak shift occurs in generalization tests following intradimensional discrimination training as a displacement of peak responding away from the S+ (a…
Descriptors: Undergraduate Students, Reinforcement, Learning Theories, Stimulus Generalization