NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Showing all 7 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
DeGutis, Joseph; Wilmer, Jeremy; Mercado, Rogelio J.; Cohan, Sarah – Cognition, 2013
Although holistic processing is thought to underlie normal face recognition ability, widely discrepant reports have recently emerged about this link in an individual differences context. Progress in this domain may have been impeded by the widespread use of subtraction scores, which lack validity due to their contamination with control condition…
Descriptors: Validity, Subtraction, Visual Perception, Regression (Statistics)
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Fayol, Michel; Thevenot, Catherine – Cognition, 2012
In a first experiment, adults were asked to solve one-digit additions, subtractions and multiplications. When the sign appeared 150 ms before the operands, addition and subtraction were solved faster than when the sign and the operands appeared simultaneously on screen. This priming effect was not observed for multiplication problems. A second…
Descriptors: Priming, Memory, Subtraction, Multiplication
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
McCrink, Koleen; Spelke, Elizabeth S. – Cognition, 2010
A dedicated, non-symbolic, system yielding imprecise representations of large quantities (approximate number system, or ANS) has been shown to support arithmetic calculations of addition and subtraction. In the present study, 5-7-year-old children without formal schooling in multiplication and division were given a task requiring a scalar…
Descriptors: Number Systems, Arithmetic, Multiplication, Young Children
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Canobi, Katherine H.; Bethune, Narelle E. – Cognition, 2008
Three studies addressed children's arithmetic. First, 50 3- to 5-year-olds judged physical demonstrations of addition, subtraction and inversion, with and without number words. Second, 20 3- to 4-year-olds made equivalence judgments of additions and subtractions. Third, 60 4- to 6-year-olds solved addition, subtraction and inversion problems that…
Descriptors: Numbers, Computation, Subtraction, Arithmetic
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Pinhas, Michal; Fischer, Martin H. – Cognition, 2008
McCrink (McCrink, Dehaene, & Dehaene-Lambertz (2007). "Moving along the number line: Operational momentum in nonsymbolic arithmetic." "Perception and Psychophysics," 69(8), 1324-1333) documented an "Operational Momentum" (OM) effect--overestimation of addition and underestimation of subtraction outcomes in non-symbolic (dot pattern) arithmetic. We…
Descriptors: Number Concepts, Subtraction, Mathematics Instruction, Cognitive Processes
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Campbell, Jamie I. D.; Metcalfe, Arron W. S. – Cognition, 2008
There is evidence for both semantic and asemantic routes for naming Arabic digits, but neuropsychological dissociations suggest that number-fact retrieval (2x3=6) can inhibit the semantic route for digit naming. Here, we tested the hypothesis that such inhibition should slow digit naming, based on the principle that reduced access to multiple…
Descriptors: Numbers, Reaction Time, Semantics, Subtraction
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Gilmore, Camilla K.; Spelke, Elizabeth S. – Cognition, 2008
In learning mathematics, children must master fundamental logical relationships, including the inverse relationship between addition and subtraction. At the start of elementary school, children lack generalized understanding of this relationship in the context of exact arithmetic problems: they fail to judge, for example, that 12 + 9 - 9 yields…
Descriptors: Elementary School Students, Preschool Children, Computation, Problem Solving