Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 0 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 3 |
Descriptor
Subtraction | 8 |
Mathematics Education | 5 |
Mathematics Instruction | 5 |
Addition | 4 |
Elementary Education | 4 |
Cognitive Processes | 3 |
Interviews | 3 |
Teaching Methods | 3 |
Algorithms | 2 |
Elementary School Students | 2 |
Foreign Countries | 2 |
More ▼ |
Source
Journal of Mathematical… | 8 |
Author
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 8 |
Reports - Research | 6 |
Information Analyses | 2 |
Education Level
Elementary Education | 2 |
Grade 1 | 1 |
Grade 3 | 1 |
Higher Education | 1 |
Audience
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Kinda, Shigehiro – Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 2013
Students are presented with problems involving three scenario types of addition and subtraction in elementary mathematics: one dynamic ("Change") and two static ("Combine, Compare"). Previous studies have indicated that the dynamic type is easier for school children, whereas the static types are more difficult and comprehended only gradually…
Descriptors: Subtraction, Mathematics Education, Addition, Foreign Countries
Murata, Aki; Kattubadi, Sailaja – Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 2012
In considering mathematics problem solving as a model-eliciting activity (Lesh & Doerr, 2003; Lesh & Harel, 2003; Lesh & Zawojewski, 2008), it is important to know "what" students are modeling for the problems: situations or solutions. This study investigated Grade 3 students' mathematization process by examining how they modeled different…
Descriptors: Problem Solving, Grade 3, Subtraction, Mathematics
Superfine, Alison Castro; Canty, Reality S.; Marshall, Anne Marie – Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 2009
The study described herein represents an initial, exploratory attempt to understand what it means to translate between external representation systems. Researchers have traditionally considered translation as an all-or-none activity. We hypothesize that translation is comprised of both knowledge and skill components, and accordingly construe…
Descriptors: Grade 1, Subtraction, Mathematics, Interviews

Streefland, Leen – Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 1996
Attempts to reconstruct the productive ideas that evolved from already existing sources for the teaching and understanding of negative numbers. Discusses examples from developmental research. (Author/MKR)
Descriptors: Concept Formation, Elementary Education, Integers, Mathematics Instruction

Gutstein, Eric; Romberg, Thomas A. – Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 1995
Reviews research using diagrams, number sentences, and algorithms to help students learn to add and subtract; poses questions on the relationship of instruction to children's knowledge construction; and proposes a research agenda in this area. (86 references) (MKR)
Descriptors: Addition, Algorithms, Arithmetic, Cognitive Processes

Schmittau, Jean – Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 1993
Based on the cognitive psychological theories of Vygotsky and Davydov, discusses the establishment of connections between mathematical elements, and the algorithmic rules that govern them, and children's spontaneous mathematical concepts. Presents examples that establish connections involving addition and subtraction, comparing numerical…
Descriptors: Addition, Cognitive Development, Cognitive Processes, Elementary Education

Schliemann, Analucia Dias; And Others – Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 1993
A 14-year-old Brazilian boy in the fourth grade was given 170 problems orally, each asking for the sum of 2 2-digit numbers. Responses indicated he had invented his own algorithm for addition, similar to the school one, based largely on his experiences with money. (Contains 13 references.) (JAF)
Descriptors: Addition, Algorithms, Case Studies, Computation

Boulton-Lewis, Gillian M. – Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 1993
Describes research which compared the processing loads of mathematical representations and strategies used by (n=3) teachers and (n=29) children aged 5-8 for interpreting subtraction operations represented in numerical form. Some representations and strategies appeared to impose an extra processing load which can interfere with conceptual…
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Elementary Education, Elementary School Students, Elementary School Teachers