Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 0 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 8 |
Descriptor
Intervals | 10 |
Task Analysis | 10 |
Cognitive Processes | 4 |
Evaluation Methods | 3 |
Memory | 3 |
Time Perspective | 3 |
Attention | 2 |
Cues | 2 |
Inhibition | 2 |
Measures (Individuals) | 2 |
Phonemes | 2 |
More ▼ |
Source
Author
Boller, Benjamin | 1 |
Caltagirone, Carlo | 1 |
Carelli, Maria Grazia | 1 |
Chan, Po Chi | 1 |
Crosbie, Jennifer | 1 |
Dieudonne, Benedicte | 1 |
Ergis, Anne-Marie | 1 |
Forman, Helen | 1 |
Grondin, Simon | 1 |
Ickowicz, Abel | 1 |
Jennings, Janine M. | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 10 |
Reports - Evaluative | 10 |
Education Level
Grade 3 | 1 |
Audience
Location
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
Conners Rating Scales | 1 |
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Pinto, Carlos; Machado, Armando – Learning and Motivation, 2011
To better understand short-term memory for temporal intervals, we re-examined the choose-short effect. In Experiment 1, to contrast the predictions of two models of this effect, the subjective shortening and the coding models, pigeons were exposed to a delayed matching-to-sample task with three sample durations (2, 6 and 18 s) and retention…
Descriptors: Intervals, Infants, Tests, Short Term Memory
Boller, Benjamin; Jennings, Janine M.; Dieudonne, Benedicte; Verny, Marc; Ergis, Anne-Marie – Brain and Cognition, 2012
Objective: This study was designed to extend the use of a memory training technique, known as the repetition-lag procedure, to Alzheimer patients. The specificity of this procedure is to target the process of recollection for improvement. Method: A group of 12 patients were trained individually for 6 h. The training procedure consisted of a series…
Descriptors: Control Groups, Intervals, Alzheimers Disease, Diseases
Oberauer, Klaus; Lange, Elke B. – Journal of Memory and Language, 2008
Reports two experiments on mechanism of interference in working memory. Experiment 1 shows that a target word in a memory list, which bears high similarity to one of 4 words read aloud in the retention interval, is recalled less well than a control word. A second target word, not similar to any word read aloud but with all its phonemes repeated…
Descriptors: Phonemes, Prediction, Memory, Verbal Communication
Rast, Rebekah – International Review of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching (IRAL), 2010
This paper examines cross-linguistic influence in third language acquisition relative to language typology, psychotypology and proficiency level. In particular, it observes how learners make use of their background languages when faced with a language they know little to nothing about. The participants, native French speakers with English as a…
Descriptors: Sentences, Intervals, Second Language Learning, Language Classification
Soreni, Noam; Crosbie, Jennifer; Ickowicz, Abel; Schachar, Russell – Journal of Attention Disorders, 2009
Objective: To measure test-retest reliability of the Stop-Signal Task (SST) and the Conners' Continuous Performance Test (CPT) in children with ADHD. Methods: 12 children with ADHD (age 11.46 plus or minus 1.66) participated in the study. Primary outcome measures were stop-signal reaction time (SSRT) for the SST and CPT's commission errors (%FP).…
Descriptors: Intervals, Reaction Time, Performance Tests, Attention Deficit Disorders
Vicario, Carmelo Mario; Caltagirone, Carlo; Oliveri, Massimiliano – Brain and Cognition, 2007
The representation of time and space are closely linked in the cognitive system. Optokinetic stimulation modulates spatial attention in healthy subjects and patients with spatial neglect. In order to evaluate whether optokinetic stimulation could influence time perception, a group of healthy subjects performed "time-comparison" tasks of sub- and…
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Attention, Evaluation Methods, Bias
Monsell, Stephen; Mizon, Guy A. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2006
In 6 task-cuing experiments, with 2 cues per task, the authors varied cue-stimulus interval to investigate G. D. Logan and C. Bundesen's (2003) claim that when cue repetition is controlled for, task-switch cost and its reduction with preparation are largely eliminated and hence cannot index an endogenous control process. Experiment 1 replicates…
Descriptors: Experimental Psychology, Models, Cues, Intervals
Mantyla, Timo; Carelli, Maria Grazia; Forman, Helen – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2007
This study examined time-based prospective memory performance in relation to individual and developmental differences in executive functioning. School-age children and young adults completed six experimental tasks that tapped three basic components of executive functioning: inhibition, updating, and mental shifting. Monitoring performance was…
Descriptors: Children, Young Adults, Memory, Cognitive Ability
Grondin, Simon – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2005
This study tested the hypothesis that memory is a major source of variance in temporal processing. Participants categorized intervals as short or long. The number of base durations and interval types mixed within blocks of trials varied from 1 session to another. Results revealed that mixing 2 base durations within blocks increased categorization…
Descriptors: Memory, Cognitive Processes, Hypothesis Testing, Intervals
Penney, Trevor B.; Leung, Kar Man; Chan, Po Chi; Meng, Xiangzhi; McBride-Chang, Catherine A. – Annals of Dyslexia, 2005
The role of information processing deficits in poor readers of nonalphabetic scripts such as Chinese is not well documented. Here, we examined perceptual processing in good and poor readers of Chinese. Specifically, two groups of third grade children comprising 20 "good readers" and 19 "poor readers," drawn from a larger pool of 254 students, were…
Descriptors: Measures (Individuals), Grade 3, Phonology, Phonemes