NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Showing all 7 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Larraza, Saioa; Samuel, Arthur G.; Oñederra, Miren Lourdes – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2016
Bilingual speakers must acquire the phonemic inventory of 2 languages and need to recognize spoken words cross-linguistically; a demanding job potentially made even more difficult due to dialectal variation, an intrinsic property of speech. The present work examines how bilinguals perceive second language (L2) accented speech and where…
Descriptors: Bilingualism, Second Language Learning, Pronunciation, Semantics
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Lemhofer, Kristin; Schriefers, Herbert; Jescheniak, Jorg D. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2006
In many languages, the production of noun phrases requires the selection of gender-marked elements like determiners or inflectional suffixes. There is a recent debate as to whether the selection of freestanding gender-marked elements, such as determiners, follows the same processing mechanisms as the selection of bound gender-marked morphemes,…
Descriptors: Uncommonly Taught Languages, Indo European Languages, Morphemes, Suffixes
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Stuart-Smith, Jane; Martin, Deirdre – International Journal of Bilingualism, 1999
Discusses development of a battery of tasks that were used to assess phonological processing skills in Panjabi-English bilingual children in West Birmingham, United Kingdom. Results support the notion that at least some tasks of phonological awareness may be language specific. (Author/VWL)
Descriptors: Bilingualism, English, Foreign Countries, Language Processing
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Lust, Barbara; Eisele, Julie – Journal of Child Language, 1991
Garman (1974), reporting on 20 Tamil children aged 3 to 5, postulated a linguistic strategy and 2 prelinguistic strategies to explain results of a question-picture choice task involving sentences with embedded and subordinate clauses. Reanalysis of this data identifies four processing strategies and argues that certain grammatical sensitivities…
Descriptors: Language Acquisition, Language Processing, Language Research, Learning Strategies
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Kaiser, Elsi; Trueswell, John C. – Cognition, 2004
On-line comprehension studies of flexible word-order languages find that noncanonical ("scrambled") structures induce more difficulty than canonical structures [e.g., Hyona & Hujanen, "Q. J. Exp. Psychol." 50A (1997) 841-858], with this difference being attributed to the structural complexity/infrequency of these forms. However, by presenting…
Descriptors: Syntax, Discourse Modes, Finno Ugric Languages, Language Processing
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Tenjovic, Lazar; Lalovic, Dejan – Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 2005
The relatedness of phonological coding to the articulatory mechanisms in visual word recognition vary in different writing systems. While articulatory suppression (i.e., continuous verbalising during a visual word processing task) has a detrimental effect on the processing of Japanese words printed in regular syllabic Khana script, it has no such…
Descriptors: Written Language, Alphabets, Word Recognition, Language Processing
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Cromdal, Jakob – Applied Psycholinguistics, 1999
In this study, metalinguistic ability is studied in terms of dual skill components: control of linguistic processing and analysis of linguistic knowledge. English-Swedish bilinguals (n=38), assigned to two groups according to relative proficiency, and 16 Swedish monolinguals, aged 6 to 7 years, received three tasks: symbol substitution,…
Descriptors: Bilingualism, English, Error Analysis (Language), Language Processing