Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 1 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 2 |
Descriptor
Syntax | 6 |
Verbs | 6 |
Language Acquisition | 5 |
Young Children | 4 |
Child Language | 3 |
Language Processing | 2 |
Nouns | 2 |
Semantics | 2 |
Toddlers | 2 |
Adverbs | 1 |
Classification | 1 |
More ▼ |
Source
Child Development | 6 |
Author
Cheung, Him | 1 |
Christophe, Anne | 1 |
Creed, Nikki | 1 |
Fisher, Cynthia | 1 |
Fiévet, Anne-Caroline | 1 |
Havron, Naomi | 1 |
Hsuan-Chih, Chen | 1 |
Kako, Edward T. | 1 |
Lempert, Henrietta | 1 |
Mo, Lei | 1 |
Naigles, Letitia G. | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 6 |
Reports - Research | 6 |
Education Level
Early Childhood Education | 1 |
Audience
Researchers | 1 |
Location
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Havron, Naomi; de Carvalho, Alex; Fiévet, Anne-Caroline; Christophe, Anne – Child Development, 2019
Adults create and update predictions about what speakers will say next. This study asks whether prediction can drive language acquisition, by testing whether 3- to 4-year-old children (n = 45) adapt to recent information when learning novel words. The study used a syntactic context which can precede both nouns and verbs to manipulate children's…
Descriptors: Prediction, Vocabulary Development, Nouns, Verbs
Yuan, Sylvia; Fisher, Cynthia; Snedeker, Jesse – Child Development, 2012
Two-year-olds use the sentence structures verbs appear in--"subcategorization frames"--to guide verb learning. This is syntactic bootstrapping. This study probed the developmental origins of this ability. The structure-mapping account proposes that children begin with a bias toward one-to-one mapping between nouns in sentences and participant…
Descriptors: Cues, Sentences, Verbs, Nouns

Naigles, Letitia G.; Kako, Edward T. – Child Development, 1993
Three experiments presented nonsense verbs to two-year-olds either in syntactic isolation or embedded within a transitive syntactic frame. Found that children had identifiable action biases in the absence of syntactic information and that these biases were shifted by the addition of a transitive syntactic frame. (MDM)
Descriptors: Language Acquisition, Semantics, Syntax, Thinking Skills

Lempert, Henrietta – Child Development, 1989
Investigates whether patient animacy affected the acquisition of the passive construction of syntax of 32 children aged two-five years. Results indicate that children who were taught the passive with animate patients produced more passives in the teaching phase than did comparable children who received inanimate patients. (RJC)
Descriptors: Child Language, Language Acquisition, Language Processing, Preschool Children

Stockman, Ida J.; Vaughn-Cooke, Fay – Child Development, 1992
Samples of the language used by 4 children were recorded longitudinally between 1.5 and 3 years of age. Children's expressions of motion were categorized into expressions involving a source, path, or goal of motion. There were developmental changes, including an increase in the use of words relating to goals as children grew older. (BC)
Descriptors: Adverbs, Language Acquisition, Lexicology, Longitudinal Studies
Cheung, Him; Hsuan-Chih, Chen; Creed, Nikki; Ng, Lisa; Ping Wang, Sui; Mo, Lei – Child Development, 2004
Complex complements are clausal objects containing tensed verbs (e.g., that she cried) or infinitives (e.g., to cry), following main verbs of communication or mental activities (e.g., say, want). This research examined whether English- and Cantonese-speaking 4-year-olds' complement understanding uniquely predicts their representation of other…
Descriptors: Verbs, Syntax, Comprehension, Cognitive Development