Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 5 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 18 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 27 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 109 |
Descriptor
Source
Author
Hathaway, Warren E. | 3 |
Bayne, Sian | 2 |
Birren, Faber | 2 |
Blackwell, H. Richard | 2 |
Crouch, C. L. | 2 |
Diana E. Gal-Szabo | 2 |
Epp, Dianne N. | 2 |
Eva M. Shivers | 2 |
Hall, Elaine | 2 |
Larissa M. Gaias | 2 |
Meltzoff, Andrew N. | 2 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Education Level
Audience
Practitioners | 22 |
Teachers | 12 |
Researchers | 6 |
Administrators | 3 |
Students | 2 |
Parents | 1 |
Location
United Kingdom | 7 |
Canada | 5 |
California | 4 |
United Kingdom (England) | 4 |
Australia | 3 |
Asia | 2 |
Colorado | 2 |
Connecticut | 2 |
Germany | 2 |
Hawaii | 2 |
Israel | 2 |
More ▼ |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Individuals with Disabilities… | 1 |
No Child Left Behind Act 2001 | 1 |
Rehabilitation Act 1973 | 1 |
Social Security | 1 |
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating

Corn, Anne L. – Journal of Visual Impairment and Blindness, 1983
A model is proposed that organizes factors related to visual function into three dimensions: visual abilities, available resources and past experiences, and environmental cues. The model is explained to be useful in determining how to elicit visual behaviors or maximize visual function in persons with low vision. (Author/CL)
Descriptors: Models, Partial Vision, Visual Environment, Visual Learning

Young, David E. – Studies in Art Education, 1982
Far from being peripheral to human functioning, aesthetic activity is fundamental to the process of coping. It is distinguished from other cultural activity, like religion and science, because its raw materials are formal properties--line, form, color, texture--derived from the external world. (Author/CS)
Descriptors: Aesthetic Values, Anthropology, Art, Culture

Fagen, Jeffrey W.; Rovee, Carolyn Kent – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1976
In two studies footkicks and visual attention of 3-month-olds were measured across daily sessions with conjugate reinforcement provided by an overhead mobile containing identical components. Results imply that infants respond relationally, actively manipulating their visual environments as a function of their previous contextual experiences. (JH)
Descriptors: Infants, Perceptual Development, Research, Rewards
Confino, Barbara – Saturday Review: Education, 1973
Description of the Visual Studies Workshop in Rochester, New York, and the philosophy of director Nathan Lyons. (SP)
Descriptors: Educational Philosophy, Photography, Special Programs, Visual Environment
Modern Schools, 1973
Desmonstrates the recommended reflectances in the classroom and suggestions are made for lighting maintenance and designing a new lighting system. Illustrated. (Author)
Descriptors: Educational Environment, Guides, Lighting, Lighting Design

Ovink, G. W. – Visible Language, 1972
Descriptors: Design, Information Processing, Photocomposition, Printing
Broadwell, Martin M. – Training Bus Ind, 1970
Descriptors: Acoustics, Classrooms, Facility Guidelines, Space Utilization
Fransecky, Roger B. – Audiovisual Instr, 1969
Descriptors: Disadvantaged Youth, Instructional Innovation, Photography, Teaching Methods
Schatz, Carla J. – Scientific American, 1992
Discusses neural activity and stimulation crucial in fetal brain development and the formation of the mind. Focuses on activity-dependent remodeling related to development of the visual system and retinal activity. (MCO)
Descriptors: Development, Eyes, Human Body, Science Education
Elwazanim, Salim A. – Educational Facility Planner, 1998
Discusses educational facility lighting management, and examines how light quantity, distribution, and quality-enhancement strategies can improve the indoor environment while reducing lighting costs. Informational tables provide lighting pattern, color, and illuminance data. (GR)
Descriptors: Educational Facilities, Elementary Secondary Education, Glare, Lighting
Wilcox, Teresa; Woods, Rebecca; Chapa, Catherine; McCurry, Sarah – Developmental Psychology, 2007
Recent research indicates that by 4.5 months, infants use shape and size information as the basis for individuating objects but that it is not until 11.5 months that they use color information for this purpose. The present experiments investigated the extent to which infants' sensitivity to color information could be increased through select…
Descriptors: Object Permanence, Infants, Visual Environment, Visual Perception
Texas Education Agency, Austin. Div. of Special Education. – 1984
The manual was intended to help school personnel (either an educator of the visually handicapped and/or a certified orientation and mobility instructor) evaluate the functional use of residual vision. The manual is organized in three major sections, which cover the following: (1) the general areas addressed in functional vision evaluation (such as…
Descriptors: Vision Tests, Visual Acuity, Visual Environment, Visual Impairments

Harris, P. L. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1974
Experiments presented indicate that perseverative error in year-old infants cannot simply be a memory problem. Possible explanations are examined. (Author/ED)
Descriptors: Infant Behavior, Infants, Inhibition, Memory

Greenberg, David J. – Child Development, 1971
Descriptors: Difficulty Level, Infants, Maturation, Visual Environment

Healy, Alice F.; Drewnowski, Adam – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 1983
Using a combination of letter-detection and proofreading techniques, subjects searching for target letters in printed text made more errors on correctly spelled words than misspelled words. This word inferiority effect contrasts with the superior perception of letters in words over nonwords commonly found in tachistoscopic studies. (Author/CM)
Descriptors: Higher Education, Letters (Alphabet), Perception, Reading Research