NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Showing all 8 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Zu, Tianlong; Hutson, John; Loschky, Lester C.; Rebello, N. Sanjay – Journal of Educational Psychology, 2020
In a previous study, DeLeeuw and Mayer (2008) found support for the triarchic model of cognitive load (Sweller, Van MerriĆ«nboer, & Paas, 1998, 2019) by showing that three different metrics could be used to independently measure 3 hypothesized types of cognitive load: intrinsic, extraneous, and germane. However, 2 of the 3 metrics that the…
Descriptors: Eye Movements, Cognitive Processes, Difficulty Level, Multimedia Instruction
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
DeLeeuw, Krista E.; Mayer, Richard E. – Journal of Educational Psychology, 2008
Understanding how to measure cognitive load is a fundamental challenge for cognitive load theory. In 2 experiments, 155 college students (ages = 17 to 22; 49 men and 106 women) with low domain knowledge learned from a multimedia lesson on electric motors. At 8 points during learning, their cognitive load was measured via self-report scales (mental…
Descriptors: Reaction Time, Rating Scales, Cognitive Processes, Experiments
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Kirchner, Grace L. – Journal of Educational Psychology, 1976
Highly active second-grade boys made fewer correct detections in a 48-minute vigilance task than did their normal peers. Detection efficiency was inversely related to interval length, and this effect was more pronounced for the highly active group. (RC)
Descriptors: Attention Control, Elementary Education, Grade 2, Hyperactivity
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Kee, Daniel W.; Nakayama, Susan Y. – Journal of Educational Psychology, 1982
Three experiments assessing the effects of nonbizarre versus bizarre pictorial elaboration on the paired-associate retention of noun pairs were conducted with kindergarten and second-grade children. Both pictorial elaborations were found to facilitate performance relative to a standard presentation condition and forgetting of pairs was equivalent.…
Descriptors: Association (Psychology), Elementary Education, Paired Associate Learning, Retention (Psychology)
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Neimark, Edith D.; Slotnick, Nan S. – Journal of Educational Psychology, 1970
Descriptors: Connected Discourse, Cross Cultural Studies, Japanese, Language Acquisition
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Gathercole, Susan E.; Pickering, Susan J. – Journal of Educational Psychology, 2000
A test battery was designed to test the Baddeley and Hitch model of working memory. The battery was administered to 87 children aged 6 and 7 years. Correlations between subtest scores indicated high construct validity for the central executive and phonological loop measures, although not for the visuospatial measures. (Contains 100 references and…
Descriptors: Construct Validity, Evaluation Methods, Foreign Countries, Learning Processes
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Mayer, Richard E.; Anderson, Richard B. – Journal of Educational Psychology, 1992
In 2 experiments, 280 college students studied animations depicting a mechanical operation with concurrent oral narration of the process, successive animation and narration, animation alone, narration alone, or no instruction (the control group). Results are consistent with a dual coding model of retention and problem solving. (SLD)
Descriptors: Animation, Coding, College Students, Comparative Analysis
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Siaw, Susan Nakayama – Journal of Educational Psychology, 1984
Using cross classifiable pictures, this study assessed taxonomic and thematic clustering in free recall in 240 children, ages seven and ten. The effects of both free and constrained test conditions and developmental, ethnic, and socioeconomic differences, were examined. (BS)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Aptitude Treatment Interaction, Cluster Grouping, Cognitive Development