Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 0 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 13 |
Descriptor
Spatial Ability | 41 |
Visual Perception | 41 |
Cognitive Processes | 14 |
Young Children | 13 |
Age Differences | 11 |
Infants | 10 |
Comparative Analysis | 7 |
Visual Discrimination | 7 |
Adults | 6 |
Children | 6 |
Elementary School Students | 6 |
More ▼ |
Source
Journal of Experimental Child… | 41 |
Author
Acredolo, Linda P. | 2 |
Barrett, Susan E. | 2 |
Bhatt, Ramesh S. | 2 |
Bremner, J. Gavin | 2 |
Hayden, Angela | 2 |
Reed, Andrea | 2 |
Shepp, Bryan E. | 2 |
Adams, Anne-Marie | 1 |
Andreasen, Gillian | 1 |
Bard, Chantal | 1 |
Batten, Annabel | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 40 |
Reports - Research | 38 |
Reports - Evaluative | 2 |
Education Level
Elementary Education | 2 |
Grade 3 | 2 |
Early Childhood Education | 1 |
Grade 1 | 1 |
Grade 2 | 1 |
Grade 4 | 1 |
Audience
Researchers | 6 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Morales, Julia; Calvo, Alejandra; Bialystok, Ellen – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2013
Two studies are reported comparing the performance of monolingual and bilingual children on tasks requiring different levels of working memory. In the first study, 56 5-year-olds performed a Simon-type task that manipulated working memory demands by comparing conditions based on two rules and four rules and manipulated conflict resolution demands…
Descriptors: Executive Function, Bilingualism, Monolingualism, Cognitive Development
Purser, Harry R. M.; Farran, Emily K.; Courbois, Yannick; Lemahieu, Axelle; Mellier, Daniel; Sockeel, Pascal; Blades, Mark – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2012
The aim of this study was to investigate route-learning ability in 67 children aged 5 to 11 years and to relate route-learning performance to the components of Baddeley's model of working memory. Children carried out tasks that included measures of verbal and visuospatial short-term memory and executive control and also measures of verbal and…
Descriptors: Virtual Classrooms, Short Term Memory, Long Term Memory, Children
Lleras, Alejandro; Porporino, Mafalda; Burack, Jacob A.; Enns, James T. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2011
In this study, 7-19-year-olds performed an interrupted visual search task in two experiments. Our question was whether the tendency to respond within 500 ms after a second glimpse of a display (the "rapid resumption" effect ["Psychological Science", 16 (2005) 684-688]) would increase with age in the same way as overall search efficiency. The…
Descriptors: Visual Stimuli, Visual Perception, Children, Adolescents
Robbins, Rachel A.; Shergill, Yaadwinder; Maurer, Daphne; Lewis, Terri L. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2011
Adults are expert at recognizing faces, in part because of exquisite sensitivity to the spacing of facial features. Children are poorer than adults at recognizing facial identity and less sensitive to spacing differences. Here we examined the specificity of the immaturity by comparing the ability of 8-year-olds, 14-year-olds, and adults to…
Descriptors: Maturity (Individuals), Nonverbal Communication, Spatial Ability, Visual Discrimination
Brunswick, Nicola; Martin, G. Neil; Rippon, Georgina – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2012
This longitudinal study examined the contribution of phonological awareness, phonological memory, and visuospatial ability to reading development in 142 English-speaking children from the start of kindergarten to the middle of Grade 2. Partial cross-lagged analyses revealed significant relationships between early performance on block design and…
Descriptors: Beginning Reading, Phonological Awareness, Reading Ability, Spatial Ability
Simmons, Fiona R.; Willis, Catherine; Adams, Anne-Marie – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2012
A comprehensive working memory battery and tests of mathematical skills were administered to 90 children--41 in Year 1 (5-6 years of age) and 49 in Year 3 (7-8 years of age). Working memory could explain statistically significant variance in number writing, magnitude judgment, and single-digit arithmetic, but the different components of working…
Descriptors: Phonology, Short Term Memory, Arithmetic, Mathematics Skills
Zheng, Xinhua; Swanson, H. Lee; Marcoulides, George A. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2011
This study determined the working memory (WM) components (executive, phonological loop, and visual-spatial sketchpad) that best predicted mathematical word problem-solving accuracy of elementary school children in Grades 2, 3, and 4 (N = 310). A battery of tests was administered to assess problem-solving accuracy, problem-solving processes, WM,…
Descriptors: Elementary School Students, Structural Equation Models, Problem Solving, Short Term Memory
Berg, Derek H. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2008
The cognitive underpinnings of arithmetic calculation in children are noted to involve working memory; however, cognitive processes related to arithmetic calculation and working memory suggest that this relationship is more complex than stated previously. The purpose of this investigation was to examine the relative contributions of processing…
Descriptors: Short Term Memory, Computation, Cognitive Processes, Arithmetic
Hayden, Angela; Bhatt, Ramesh S.; Reed, Andrea; Corbly, Christine R.; Joseph, Jane E. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2007
Sensitivity to second-order relational information (i.e., spatial relations among features such as the distance between eyes) is a vital part of achieving expertise with face processing. Prior research is unclear on whether infants are sensitive to second-order differences seen in typical human populations. In the current experiments, we examined…
Descriptors: Infants, Visual Perception, Females, Whites
Durand, Karine; Gallay, Mathieu; Seigneuric, Alix; Robichon, Fabrice; Baudouin, Jean-Yves – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2007
The development of children's ability to recognize facial emotions and the role of configural information in this development were investigated. In the study, 100 5-, 7-, 9-, and 11-year-olds and 26 adults needed to recognize the emotion displayed by upright and upside-down faces. The same participants needed to recognize the emotion displayed by…
Descriptors: Nonverbal Communication, Psychological Patterns, Recognition (Psychology), Individual Development
Landerl, Karin; Fussenegger, Barbara; Moll, Kristina; Willburger, Edith – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2009
This study tests the hypothesis that dyslexia and dyscalculia are associated with two largely independent cognitive deficits, namely a phonological deficit in the case of dyslexia and a deficit in the number module in the case of dyscalculia. In four groups of 8- to 10-year-olds (42 control, 21 dyslexic, 20 dyscalculic, and 26…
Descriptors: Reading Difficulties, Dyslexia, Phonological Awareness, Short Term Memory

Acredolo, Linda P.; And Others – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1984
Investigates the role active, self-produced movement might play in the type of rotation task typically used to assess spatial orientation in children 12 months to 18 months of age. Results indicated that, at least at 12 months, spatial orientation was indeed facilitated by allowing the infants (n = 13) to move through space on their own. (RH)
Descriptors: Infants, Perception, Spatial Ability, Visual Perception
Bhatt, Ramesh S.; Hayden, Angela; Reed, Andrea; Bertin, Evelin; Joseph, Jane – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2006
Object parts are signaled by concave discontinuities in shape contours. In seven experiments, we examined whether 5- and 6 1/2-month-olds are sensitive to concavities as special aspects of contours. Infants of both ages detected discrepant concave elements amid convex distractors but failed to discriminate convex elements among concave…
Descriptors: Infants, Perception, Child Psychology, Visual Discrimination

And Others; Braine, Lila Ghent – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1980
This study tested whether the first level in processing orientation information results in perceiving whether a shape is upright or nonupright. Theory states that nonupright orientations are not distinguished from each other. As predicted, three- and four-year-olds discriminated upright from nonupright pictures more readily than they discriminated…
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Preschool Children, Spatial Ability, Visual Discrimination

Tyler, D.; McKenzie, B. E. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1990
Examined infants' ability to locate an invisible target after instrumental and association training. After instrumental training, localization was dependent on visual cues, whereas after association training, it occurred with or without visual cues. Concluded that an updating strategy based on proprioceptive information is operative from the…
Descriptors: Developmental Stages, Infant Behavior, Learning Strategies, Spatial Ability