Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 8 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 30 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 83 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 278 |
Descriptor
Cues | 403 |
Visual Perception | 403 |
Visual Stimuli | 153 |
Cognitive Processes | 105 |
Attention | 104 |
Spatial Ability | 91 |
Foreign Countries | 69 |
Eye Movements | 63 |
Comparative Analysis | 56 |
Reaction Time | 54 |
Task Analysis | 51 |
More ▼ |
Source
Author
Theeuwes, Jan | 4 |
Burack, Jacob A. | 3 |
Deruelle, Christine | 3 |
Henik, Avishai | 3 |
Hollingworth, Andrew | 3 |
Iarocci, Grace | 3 |
Lee, Kang | 3 |
Mou, Weimin | 3 |
Olivers, Christian N. L. | 3 |
Pratt, Jay | 3 |
Ropar, Danielle | 3 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Education Level
Higher Education | 59 |
Postsecondary Education | 33 |
Elementary Education | 7 |
Early Childhood Education | 3 |
Preschool Education | 2 |
Adult Education | 1 |
Grade 2 | 1 |
Grade 4 | 1 |
Grade 7 | 1 |
Junior High Schools | 1 |
Middle Schools | 1 |
More ▼ |
Audience
Practitioners | 2 |
Researchers | 2 |
Teachers | 2 |
Location
Germany | 8 |
China | 7 |
Australia | 6 |
Canada | 5 |
France | 5 |
Brazil | 4 |
California | 4 |
United Kingdom | 4 |
Italy | 3 |
Pennsylvania | 3 |
Taiwan | 3 |
More ▼ |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Jeff Moher; Anna Delos Reyes; Trafton Drew – Cognitive Research: Principles and Implications, 2024
Irrelevant salient distractors can trigger early quitting in visual search, causing observers to miss targets they might otherwise find. Here, we asked whether task-relevant salient cues can produce a similar early quitting effect on the subset of trials where those cues fail to highlight the target. We presented participants with a difficult…
Descriptors: Attention, Cues, Environmental Influences, Visual Perception
Jesse Q. Sargent; Lauren L. Richmond; Devin M. Kellis; Maverick E. Smith; Jeffrey M. Zacks – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2024
Spatial memory is important for supporting the successful completion of everyday activities and is a particularly vulnerable domain in late life. Grouping items together in memory, or chunking, can improve spatial memory performance. In memory for desktop scale spaces and well-learned large-scale environments, error patterns suggest that…
Descriptors: Spatial Ability, Memory, Cognitive Processes, Aging (Individuals)
Luo, Tianrui; Huang, Liqiang; Tian, Mi – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2023
The retro-cue effect (RCE) describes the finding that participants' working memory performance is enhanced when their attention is directed to the to-be-tested position by a spatial cue during the retention interval. Here, we explore the relationship between RCE and working memory consolidation. A sequential display retro-cue paradigm is used for…
Descriptors: Cues, Recall (Psychology), Short Term Memory, Attention
Liu, Chunyan; Zhai, Huajie; Su, Shuhua; Song, Sutao; Chen, Gongxiang; Jiang, Yi – Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 2022
Previous studies have found reduced leftward bias of facial processing in individuals with Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD). However, it is not clear whether they manifest a leftward bias in general visual processing. To shed light on this issue, the current study used the manual line bisection task to assess children 5 to 15 years of age with ASD…
Descriptors: Autism Spectrum Disorders, Children, Adolescents, Visual Perception
I. R. Jackson; E. Perugia; M. A. Stone; G. H. Saunders – Cognitive Research: Principles and Implications, 2024
The use of face coverings can make communication more difficult by removing access to visual cues as well as affecting the physical transmission of speech sounds. This study aimed to assess the independent and combined contributions of visual and auditory cues to impaired communication when using face coverings. In an online task, 150 participants…
Descriptors: Verbal Communication, Cues, Auditory Perception, Visual Perception
Ke, Shih-Chiang; Gupta, Ankit; Lo, Yu-Hui; Ting, Chih-Chung; Tseng, Philip – Cognitive Research: Principles and Implications, 2023
The FedEx logo makes clever use of figure-ground ambiguity to create an "invisible" arrow in the background space between "E" and "x". Most designers believe the hidden arrow can convey an unconscious impression of speed and precision about the FedEx brand, which may influence subsequent behavior. To test this…
Descriptors: Visual Perception, Cues, Cognitive Processes, Prior Learning
Feng, Shuyuan; Wang, Qiandong; Hu, Yixiao; Lu, Haoyang; Li, Tianbi; Song, Ci; Fang, Jing; Chen, Lihan; Yi, Li – Developmental Science, 2023
Autistic children (AC) show less audiovisual speech integration in the McGurk task, which correlates with their reduced mouth-looking time. The present study examined whether AC's less audiovisual speech integration in the McGurk task could be increased by increasing their mouth-looking time. We recruited 4- to 8-year-old AC and nonautistic…
Descriptors: Autism Spectrum Disorders, Children, Speech, Auditory Perception
Mahalakshmi Ramamurthy; Alex L. White; Jason D. Yeatman – Developmental Science, 2024
In the search for mechanisms that contribute to dyslexia, the term "attention" has been invoked to explain performance in a variety of tasks, creating confusion since all tasks do, indeed, demand "attention." Many studies lack an experimental manipulation of attention that would be necessary to determine its influence on task…
Descriptors: Children, Adolescents, Dyslexia, Spatial Ability
Krasnoff, Julia; Oberauer, Klaus – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2023
This work investigates how people make judgments about the content of their visual working memory (VWM). Some studies on long-term memory suggest that people base those metacognitive judgments on the outcome of a retrieval attempt. In contrast, Son and Metcalfe (2005) observed that people identify poorly remembered items immediately, presumably by…
Descriptors: Metacognition, Short Term Memory, Visual Perception, Color
Zhang, Ziyao; Carlisle, Nancy B. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2023
Can we use attentional control to ignore known distractor features? Providing cues before a visual search trial about an upcoming distractor color (negative cue) can lead to reaction time benefits compared with no cue trials. This suggests top-down control may use negative templates to actively suppress distractor features, a notion that…
Descriptors: Attention Control, Cues, Visual Perception, Interference (Learning)
Guo, Dong; Wang, Yudan; Liao, Yifan; Li, Jiaofeng; Zhang, Xingyi; Gao, Zaifeng; Shen, Mowei; He, Jie – Child Development, 2022
Visual working memory (WM) plays a pivotal role in integrating fragments into meaningful units, but no study has addressed how visual WM integration takes place in children. The current study examined whether WM integration emerges once preschoolers master Gestalt cue and can retain two representations in WM (automatic integration hypothesis), or…
Descriptors: Short Term Memory, Visual Perception, Age Differences, Cues
Weiping Wang; Zhifan Li; Xin Lin; Yu-Hao P. Sun; Zhe Wang; Yong Wang – Cognitive Research: Principles and Implications, 2024
Facial features are important sources of information about perceived trustworthiness. Masks and protective clothing diminish the visibility of facial cues by either partially concealing the mouth and nose or covering the entire face. During the pandemic, the use of personal protective equipment affected and redefined who trusts whom in society.…
Descriptors: Clothing, Recognition (Psychology), Trust (Psychology), Human Body
Sisk, Caitlin A.; Interrante, Victoria; Jiang, Yuhong V. – Cognitive Research: Principles and Implications, 2021
When a visual search target frequently appears in one target-rich region of space, participants learn to search there first, resulting in faster reaction time when the target appears there than when it appears elsewhere. Most research on this location probability learning (LPL) effect uses 2-dimensional (2D) search environments that are distinct…
Descriptors: Spatial Ability, Probability, Visual Stimuli, Learning Processes
Stefanie Peykarjou; Stefanie Hoehl; Sabina Pauen – Child Development, 2024
This study investigated the development of rapid visual object categorization. N = 20 adults (Experiment 1), N = 21 five to six-year-old children (Experiment 2), and N = 140 four-, seven-, and eleven-month-old infants (Experiment 3; all predominantly White, 81 females, data collected in 2013-2020) participated in a fast periodic visual stimulation…
Descriptors: Cues, Visual Perception, Child Development, Infants
Verhaar, Erik; Medendorp, Wijbrand Pieter; Hunnius, Sabine; Stapel, Janny C. – Developmental Science, 2022
If cues from different sensory modalities share the same cause, their information can be integrated to improve perceptual precision. While it is well established that adults exploit sensory redundancy by integrating cues in a Bayes optimal fashion, whether children under 8 years of age combine sensory information in a similar fashion is still…
Descriptors: Bayesian Statistics, Causal Models, Statistical Inference, Visual Perception