Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 0 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 28 |
Descriptor
Vocabulary Development | 42 |
Language Acquisition | 28 |
Child Language | 10 |
Infants | 10 |
Language Processing | 10 |
Cognitive Processes | 9 |
Semantics | 8 |
Word Recognition | 8 |
Classification | 7 |
Toddlers | 7 |
Verbs | 7 |
More ▼ |
Source
Cognition | 42 |
Author
Smith, Linda B. | 4 |
Barner, David | 2 |
Borovsky, Arielle | 2 |
Bowers, Jeffrey S. | 2 |
Musolino, Julien | 2 |
Afonso, Olivia | 1 |
Aviles, Alberto | 1 |
Bale, Alan | 1 |
Bauernschmidt, Althea | 1 |
Benitez, Viridiana L. | 1 |
Bergelson, Elika | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 41 |
Reports - Research | 32 |
Reports - Evaluative | 5 |
Opinion Papers | 2 |
Reports - Descriptive | 2 |
Information Analyses | 1 |
Audience
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Wagner, Katie; Dobkins, Karen; Barner, David – Cognition, 2013
Most current accounts of color word acquisition propose that the delay between children's first production of color words and adult-like understanding is due to problems abstracting color as a domain of meaning. Here we present evidence against this hypothesis, and show that, from the time children produce color words in a labeling task they use…
Descriptors: Color, Vocabulary Development, Children, Definitions
Dumay, Nicolas; Gaskell, M. Gareth – Cognition, 2012
Two experiments explored the consolidation of spoken words, and assessed whether post-sleep novel competitor effects truly reflect engagement of these novel words in competition for lexical segmentation. Two types of competitor relationships were contrasted: the onset-aligned case (such as "frenzylk"), where the novel word is a close variant of…
Descriptors: Inhibition, Vocabulary Development, Experiments, Word Recognition
Feldman, Naomi H.; Myers, Emily B.; White, Katherine S.; Griffiths, Thomas L.; Morgan, James L. – Cognition, 2013
Infants begin to segment words from fluent speech during the same time period that they learn phonetic categories. Segmented words can provide a potentially useful cue for phonetic learning, yet accounts of phonetic category acquisition typically ignore the contexts in which sounds appear. We present two experiments to show that, contrary to the…
Descriptors: Phonetics, Infants, Cues, Adults
Bergelson, Elika; Swingley, Daniel – Cognition, 2013
Young infants' learning of words for abstract concepts like "all gone" and "eat," in contrast to their learning of more concrete words like "apple" and "shoe," may follow a relatively protracted developmental course. We examined whether infants know such abstract words. Parents named one of two events shown in side-by-side videos while their…
Descriptors: Cognitive Ability, Video Technology, Infants, Vocabulary Development
Bion, Ricardo A. H.; Borovsky, Arielle; Fernald, Anne – Cognition, 2013
When hearing a novel name, children tend to select a novel object rather than a familiar one, a bias known as disambiguation. Using online processing measures with 18-, 24-, and 30-month-olds, we investigate how the development of this bias relates to word learning. Children's proportion of looking time to a novel object after hearing a novel name…
Descriptors: Language Acquisition, Novels, Vocabulary Development, Infants
Benitez, Viridiana L.; Smith, Linda B. – Cognition, 2012
Expectancy-based localized attention has been shown to promote the formation and retrieval of multisensory memories in adults. Three experiments show that these processes also characterize attention and learning in 16- to 18-month old infants and, moreover, that these processes may play a critical role in supporting early object name learning. The…
Descriptors: Infants, Object Permanence, Prediction, Language Acquisition
Marquis, Alexandra; Shi, Rushen – Cognition, 2012
How do children learn the internal structure of inflected words? We hypothesized that bound functional morphemes begin to be encoded at the preverbal stage, driven by their frequent occurrence with highly variable roots, and that infants in turn use these morphemes to interpret other words with the same inflections. Using a preferential looking…
Descriptors: Semantics, Form Classes (Languages), Morphemes, Infants
Scott, Rose M.; Fisher, Cynthia – Cognition, 2012
Recent evidence shows that children can use cross-situational statistics to learn new object labels under referential ambiguity (e.g., Smith & Yu, 2008). Such evidence has been interpreted as support for proposals that statistical information about word-referent co-occurrence plays a powerful role in word learning. But object labels represent only…
Descriptors: Evidence, Sentences, Verbs, Figurative Language
Floccia, Caroline; Luche, Claire Delle; Durrant, Samantha; Butler, Joseph; Goslin, Jeremy – Cognition, 2012
The recognition of familiar words was evaluated in 20-month-old children raised in a rhotic accent environment to parents that had either rhotic or non-rhotic accents. Using an Intermodal Preferential Looking task children were presented with familiar objects (e.g. "bird") named in their rhotic or non-rhotic form. Children were only able to…
Descriptors: Language Acquisition, Vocabulary Development, Pronunciation, Toddlers
Borovsky, Arielle; Kutas, Marta; Elman, Jeff – Cognition, 2010
Humans have the remarkable capacity to learn words from a single instance. The goal of this study was to examine the impact of initial learning context on the understanding of novel word usage using event-related brain potentials. Participants saw known and unknown words in strongly or weakly constraining sentence contexts. After each sentence…
Descriptors: Sentences, Verbs, Vocabulary Development, Learning Processes
Saji, Noburo; Imai, Mutsumi; Saalbach, Henrik; Zhang, Yuping; Shu, Hua; Okada, Hiroyuki – Cognition, 2011
This paper explores the process through which children sort out the relations among verbs belonging to the same semantic domain. Using a set of Chinese verbs denoting a range of action events that are labeled by carrying or holding in English as a test case, we looked at how Chinese-speaking 3-, 5-, and 7-year-olds and adults apply 13 different…
Descriptors: Semantics, Verbs, Vocabulary Development, Chinese
de Marchena, Ashley; Eigsti, Inge-Marie; Worek, Amanda; Ono, Kim Emiko; Snedeker, Jesse – Cognition, 2011
While there is ample evidence that children treat words as mutually exclusive, the cognitive basis of this bias is widely debated. We focus on the distinction between pragmatic and lexical constraints accounts. High-functioning children with autism spectrum disorders (ASD) offer a unique perspective on this debate, as they acquire substantial…
Descriptors: Autism, Pervasive Developmental Disorders, Cognitive Processes, Learning Processes
St. Clair, Michelle C.; Monaghan, Padraic; Christiansen, Morten H. – Cognition, 2010
Numerous distributional cues in the child's environment may potentially assist in language learning, but what cues are useful to the child and when are these cues utilised? We propose that the most useful source of distributional cue is a flexible frame surrounding the word, where the language learner integrates information from the preceding and…
Descriptors: Cues, Grammar, Information Sources, Language Acquisition
Singh, Leher – Cognition, 2008
Although infants begin to encode and track novel words in fluent speech by 7.5 months, their ability to recognize words is somewhat limited at this stage. In particular, when the surface form of a word is altered, by changing the gender or affective prosody of the speaker, infants begin to falter at spoken word recognition. Given that natural…
Descriptors: Infants, Word Recognition, Child Development, Speech Communication
Barner, David; Brooks, Neon; Bale, Alan – Cognition, 2011
When faced with a sentence like, "Some of the toys are on the table", adults, but not preschoolers, compute a scalar implicature, taking the sentence to imply that not all the toys are on the table. This paper explores the hypothesis that children fail to compute scalar implicatures because they lack knowledge of relevant scalar alternatives to…
Descriptors: Context Effect, Sentences, Role, Inferences