NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Publication Date
In 20250
Since 20240
Since 2021 (last 5 years)0
Since 2016 (last 10 years)0
Since 2006 (last 20 years)146
Source
Journal of Experimental Child…356
Audience
Researchers41
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
Wechsler Intelligence Scale…1
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Showing 1 to 15 of 356 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Conry-Murray, Clare – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2013
Two age groups of children, 5- and 6-year-olds (n = 30) and 8- and 9-year-olds (n = 26), made judgments about which of two items a character should choose: a gender-typical item or a gender-atypical item that was preferred by the character. Judgments were made about situations where the character was (a) in a familiar public setting and (b) in a…
Descriptors: Personality, Familiarity, Age Groups, Children
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Goldenberg, Elizabeth R.; Sandhofer, Catherine M. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2013
Children have a difficult time in generalizing among changes in background context. We examined the role of two processes that may aid in generalizing category labels in new contexts. In this study, 2-year-old children were taught novel object categories in one type of contextual condition and were tested for category generalization in a new…
Descriptors: Generalization, Language Acquisition, Young Children, Experiments
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Matheson, Heath; Moore, Chris; Akhtar, Nameera – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2013
From the first year of life, imitative learning readily occurs in contexts where a demonstrator directly interacts with infants (i.e., "interactive contexts"), and at least by 18 months, imitation will also occur in third-party or observational contexts where infants witness a demonstration by another person that is not directed at them. However,…
Descriptors: Socialization, Social Cognition, Imitation, Infants
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Deng, W.; Sloutsky, Vladimir M. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2013
What is the role of linguistic labels in inductive generalization? According to one approach labels denote categories and differ from object features, whereas according to another approach labels start out as features and may become category markers in the course of development. This issue was addressed in four experiments with 4- and 5-year-olds…
Descriptors: Linguistics, Classification, Logical Thinking, Generalization
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Melis, Alicia P.; Altrichter, Kristin; Tomasello, Michael – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2013
Recent studies have shown that in situations where resources have been acquired collaboratively, children at around 3 years of age share mostly equally. We investigated 3-year-olds' sharing behavior with a collaborating partner and a free-riding partner who explicitly expressed her preference not to collaborate. Children shared more equally with…
Descriptors: Resource Allocation, Sharing Behavior, Young Children, Toddlers
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Lipko-Speed, Amanda R. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2013
Preschoolers persistently predict that they will perform better than they actually can perform on a picture recall task. The current investigation sought to explore a condition under which young children might be able to improve their predictive accuracy. Namely, children were asked to predict their recall twice for the same set of items.…
Descriptors: Accuracy, Prediction, Preschool Children, Young Children
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Wandrey, Lindsay; Quas, Jodi A.; Lyon, Thomas D. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2012
Early deceptive behavior often involves acts of wrongdoings on the part of children. As a result, it has often been assumed, although not tested directly, that children are better at identifying lies about wrongdoing than lies about other activities. We tested this assumption in two studies. In Study 1, 67 3- to 5-year-olds viewed vignettes in…
Descriptors: Deception, Ethics, Identification, Young Children
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Bechtel, Sabrina; Jeschonek, Susanna; Pauen, Sabina – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2013
This study investigated cognitive processes underlying tool use and knowledge transfer in 24-month-olds (N = 123). Following a demonstration, participants chose a tool to reach a reward in a training transfer paradigm. Differing from previous research, various aspects considered to be relevant for children's performance were integrated within the…
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Conflict, Feedback (Response), Transfer of Training
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Pathman, Thanujeni; Bauer, Patricia J. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2013
The first years of life are witness to rapid changes in long-term recall ability. In the current research we contributed to an explanation of the changes by testing the absolute and relative contributions to long-term recall of encoding and post-encoding processes. Using elicited imitation, we sampled the status of 16-, 20-, and 24-month-old…
Descriptors: Infants, Long Term Memory, Memory, Recall (Psychology)
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Einav, Shiri; Robinson, Elizabeth J.; Fox, Amy – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2013
The ability to read opens up the possibility of learning about the world indirectly via print sources, providing a powerful new opportunity for children who have for years learned effectively from what people tell them. We compared children's trust in printed versus oral information. We also examined whether children who showed preferential trust…
Descriptors: Accuracy, Emergent Literacy, Reading Ability, Reading Instruction
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Benenson, Joyce F.; Quinn, Amanda; Stella, Sandra – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2012
Evidence from ethnographic, observational, and experimental studies with humans converges to suggest that males affiliate more than females with unrelated, familiar same-sex peers, but this has never been examined directly. With this aim, we compared frequency of affiliation with a single, randomly chosen, familiar same-sex peer for the two sexes…
Descriptors: Gender Differences, Peer Relationship, Young Children, Social Behavior
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Riviere, James; David, Elodie – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2013
In the C-not-B task, 2.5-year-old children tend to look for an object in a location to which the hiding agent moved his hand (C) after moving an object from A to B. In three experiments, the authors investigated the nature of the constraints underlying toddlers' performance in this task. In Experiment 1, 2.5-year-olds were tested in a new version…
Descriptors: Young Children, Object Permanence, Toddlers, Experiments
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Godwin, Karrie E.; Matlen, Bryan J.; Fisher, Anna V. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2013
Category-based reasoning is central to mature cognition; however, the developmental course of this ability remains contested. One strong indicator of category-based reasoning is the propensity to make inferences based on semantically similar labels. Recent evidence indicates that in preschool-age children the effects of semantically similar labels…
Descriptors: Children, Priming, Family Relationship, Inferences
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Fisher, Anna; Thiessen, Erik; Godwin, Karrie; Kloos, Heidi; Dickerson, John – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2013
Selective sustained attention (SSA) is crucial for higher order cognition. Factors promoting SSA are described as exogenous or endogenous. However, there is little research specifying how these factors interact during development, due largely to the paucity of developmentally appropriate paradigms. We report findings from a novel paradigm designed…
Descriptors: Psychometrics, Models, Preschool Children, Young Children
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Riggs, Kevin J.; Jolley, Richard P.; Simpson, Andrew – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2013
We investigated the role of inhibitory control in young children's human figure drawing. We used the Bear-Dragon task as a measure of inhibitory control and used the classification system devised by Cox and Parkin to measure the development of human figure drawing. We tested 50 children aged between 40 and 64 months. Regression analysis showed…
Descriptors: Inhibition, Classification, Young Children, Freehand Drawing
Previous Page | Next Page ยป
Pages: 1  |  2  |  3  |  4  |  5  |  6  |  7  |  8  |  9  |  10  |  11  |  ...  |  24