Publication Date
In 2025 | 1 |
Since 2024 | 1 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 2 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 6 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 25 |
Descriptor
Classification | 60 |
Cognitive Development | 60 |
Young Children | 60 |
Child Development | 18 |
Concept Formation | 17 |
Age Differences | 16 |
Cognitive Processes | 14 |
Adults | 9 |
Early Childhood Education | 8 |
Developmental Stages | 6 |
Foreign Countries | 6 |
More ▼ |
Source
Author
Gelman, Susan A. | 5 |
Smith, Linda B. | 3 |
Kalish, Charles W. | 2 |
Namy, Laura L. | 2 |
Abelev, Maxim | 1 |
Adams, Alison K. | 1 |
Ahn, Woo-kyoung | 1 |
Akande, Adebowale | 1 |
Ambady, Nalini | 1 |
Amsterlaw, Jennifer A. | 1 |
Andrews, Glenda | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 51 |
Reports - Research | 46 |
Reports - Evaluative | 4 |
Opinion Papers | 3 |
Guides - Classroom - Teacher | 2 |
Information Analyses | 2 |
Speeches/Meeting Papers | 2 |
Books | 1 |
Guides - Non-Classroom | 1 |
Reports - Descriptive | 1 |
Education Level
Early Childhood Education | 7 |
Elementary Education | 6 |
Preschool Education | 3 |
Kindergarten | 2 |
Primary Education | 2 |
Elementary Secondary Education | 1 |
Grade 1 | 1 |
Grade 3 | 1 |
Grade 4 | 1 |
Grade 5 | 1 |
Audience
Researchers | 5 |
Practitioners | 1 |
Teachers | 1 |
Location
Australia | 3 |
Florida | 1 |
Iceland | 1 |
Israel | 1 |
Ohio (Columbus) | 1 |
Switzerland | 1 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
Wechsler Preschool and… | 4 |
Developmental Quotient | 1 |
Digit Span Test | 1 |
Stanford Early School… | 1 |
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Dankiw, Kylie A.; Baldock, Katherine L.; Kumar, Saravana; Tsiros, Margarita D. – Australasian Journal of Early Childhood, 2021
Identifying and describing children's play behaviours is an important component of evaluating child development. The Behaviour Mapping Schedule is a direct observational tool which aims to describe and quantify children's play behaviours but is yet to undergo reliability testing. This study aimed to determine the intra- and inter-rater reliability…
Descriptors: Interrater Reliability, Classification, Child Behavior, Play
Blanco, Nathaniel J.; Sloutsky, Vladimir M. – Developmental Psychology, 2019
Previous research has shown that when learning categories, adults and young children allocate attention differently. Adults tend to attend selectively, focusing primarily on the most relevant information, whereas young children tend to distribute their attention broadly. Although selective attention is useful in many situations, it also has costs.…
Descriptors: Young Children, Adults, Attention, Classification
Emotion Words, Emotion Concepts, and Emotional Development in Children: A Constructionist Hypothesis
Hoemann, Katie; Xu, Fei; Barrett, Lisa Feldman – Developmental Psychology, 2019
In this article, we integrate two constructionist approaches--the theory of constructed emotion and rational constructivism--to introduce several novel hypotheses for understanding emotional development. We first discuss the hypothesis that emotion categories are abstract and conceptual, whose instances share a goal-based function in a particular…
Descriptors: Emotional Development, Child Development, Psychological Patterns, Vocabulary
Taylor Lesner; Ben Clarke; Derek Kosty; Geovanna Rodriguez; Elizabeth L. Budd; Christian Doabler – Grantee Submission, 2025
This secondary analysis of data from a randomized control trial of an early mathematics intervention, ROOTS, explored whether patterns of intervention response were best categorized by the typical response/non-response binary or a more complex framework with additional response profiles. Participants included kindergarten students at risk for…
Descriptors: Mathematics Instruction, Response to Intervention, At Risk Students, Kindergarten
Milburn, Trelani F.; Lonigan, Christopher J.; Phillips, Beth M. – Journal of Learning Disabilities, 2019
The current study investigated the stability of children's risk status across the preschool year. A total of 1,102 preschool children attending Title 1 schools (n = 631) and non-Title 1 schools (n = 471) participated in this study. Using averaged standard scores for two measures of language, print knowledge, and phonological awareness administered…
Descriptors: Preschool Children, Phonological Awareness, At Risk Students, Disadvantaged Schools
Tzuriel, David; Isman, Esther B.; Klung, Tamar; Haywood, H. Carl – Journal of Cognitive Education and Psychology, 2017
Children, 4-6 years of age, in special education kindergartens were randomly assigned to a classification training (n 45) and a comparison (n 49) group. Children in the training group were taught the Classification unit of Bright Start, whereas those in the comparison group received a regular content-oriented curriculum. Both groups were given…
Descriptors: Young Children, Kindergarten, Special Education, Students with Disabilities
Weisberg, Deena Skolnick; Sobel, David M. – Cognitive Development, 2012
Can young children discriminate impossible events, which cannot happen in reality, from improbable events, which are unfamiliar but could possibly happen in reality? When asked explicitly to categorize these types of events, 4-year-olds (N = 54) tended to report that improbable events were impossible, consistent with prior results (Shtulman &…
Descriptors: Young Children, Cognitive Ability, Cognitive Development, Classification
Martarelli, Corinna S.; Mast, Fred W. – Journal of Cognition and Development, 2013
Children aged 3 to 8 years old and adults were tested on a reality–fantasy distinction task. They had to judge whether particular entities were real or fantastical, and response times were collected. We further manipulated whether the entity is a specific character or a generic fantastical entity. The results indicate that children, unlike adults,…
Descriptors: Young Children, Adults, Fantasy, Realism
Schmidt, Marco F. H.; Rakoczy, Hannes; Tomasello, Michael – Cognition, 2012
To become cooperative members of their cultural groups, developing children must follow their group's social norms. But young children are not just blind norm followers, they are also active norm enforcers, for example, protesting and correcting when someone plays a conventional game the "wrong" way. In two studies, we asked whether young children…
Descriptors: Young Children, Norms, Child Development, Games
Tek, Saime; Jaffery, Gul; Swensen, Lauren; Fein, Deborah; Naigles, Letitia R. – Cognitive Development, 2012
Previous research has demonstrated that visual properties of objects can affect shape-based categorization in a novel-name extension task; however, we still do not know how a relationship between visual properties of objects affects judgments in a novel-name extension task. We examined effects of increased visual similarity among the target and…
Descriptors: Task Analysis, Cognitive Development, Visual Stimuli, Adults
Sera, Maria D.; Gordon Millett, Katherine – Cognitive Development, 2011
Considerable evidence indicates that shape similarity plays a major role in object recognition, identification and categorization. However, little is known about shape processing and its development. Across four experiments, we addressed two related questions. First, what makes objects similar in shape? Second, how does the processing of shape…
Descriptors: Infants, Cognitive Development, Cognitive Processes, Role
Fisher, Anna V. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2011
Two experiments tested a hypothesis that reducing demands on executive control in a Dimensional Change Card Sort task will lead to improved performance in 3-year-olds. In Experiment 1, the shape dimension was represented by two dissimilar values ("stars" and "flowers"), and the color dimension was represented by two similar values ("red" and…
Descriptors: Preschool Children, Experimental Psychology, Classification, Task Analysis
Buss, Aaron T.; Spencer, John P. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2012
The Dimensional Change Card Sort (DCCS) task requires children to switch from sorting cards based on shape or color to sorting based on the other dimension. Typically, 3-year-olds perseverate, whereas 4-year-olds flexibly sort by different dimensions. Zelazo and colleagues (1996, Cognitive Development, 11, 37-63) asked children questions about the…
Descriptors: Cues, Games, Behavior Standards, Cognitive Development
Bornstein, Marc H.; Arterberry, Martha E. – Developmental Psychology, 2010
Multiple levels of category inclusiveness in 4 object domains (animals, vehicles, fruit, and furniture) were examined using a sequential touching procedure and assessed in both individual and group analyses in eighty 12-, 18-, 24-, and 30-month-olds. The roles of stimulus discriminability and child motor development, fatigue, and actions were also…
Descriptors: Young Children, Classification, Motor Development, Cognitive Processes
Meunier, Benjamin; Cordier, Francoise – Cognitive Development, 2009
The present study investigated the role of the causal status of features and feature type in biological categorizations by young children. Study 1 showed that 5-year-olds are more strongly influenced by causal features than effect features; 4-year-olds exhibit no such tendency. There therefore appears to be a conceptual change between the ages of…
Descriptors: Classification, Biology, Developmental Stages, Young Children