Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 4 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 9 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 16 |
Descriptor
Language Acquisition | 59 |
Learning Processes | 59 |
Young Children | 59 |
Child Language | 20 |
Language Research | 12 |
Cognitive Development | 11 |
Bilingualism | 9 |
Early Childhood Education | 9 |
Psycholinguistics | 8 |
Second Language Learning | 8 |
Child Development | 7 |
More ▼ |
Source
Author
Publication Type
Education Level
Early Childhood Education | 9 |
Preschool Education | 4 |
Elementary Education | 1 |
Kindergarten | 1 |
Primary Education | 1 |
Audience
Policymakers | 1 |
Practitioners | 1 |
Researchers | 1 |
Teachers | 1 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
No Child Left Behind Act 2001 | 1 |
Assessments and Surveys
Raven Progressive Matrices | 1 |
Wechsler Preschool and… | 1 |
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Weiler, Brian K.; Decker, Allyson L. – Communication Disorders Quarterly, 2022
To explore the relationship between socioeconomic status (SES) and language domain (vocabulary, syntax, process), the QUILS was administered to 212 kindergartners. Children from very-high poverty schools performed significantly below children from high poverty and mid-low poverty schools. SES impacts language-learning processes (i.e., fast…
Descriptors: Socioeconomic Status, Language Usage, Vocabulary, Syntax
Fort, Mathilde; Lammertink, Imme; Peperkamp, Sharon; Guevara-Rukoz, Adriana; Fikkert, Paula; Tsuji, Sho – Developmental Science, 2018
Adults and toddlers systematically associate pseudowords such as "bouba" and "kiki" with round and spiky shapes, respectively, a sound symbolic phenomenon known as the "bouba-kiki effect." To date, whether this sound symbolic effect is a property of the infant brain present at birth or is a learned aspect of language…
Descriptors: Acoustics, Infants, Brain, Language Acquisition
White, Michelle Jennifer; Southwood, Frenette; Huddlestone, Kate – First Language, 2023
Afrikaans is a West Germanic language that originated in South Africa as a descendent of Dutch. It displays discontinuous sentential negation (SN), where negation is expressed by two phonologically identical negative particles that appear in two different positions in the sentence. The negation system is argued to be an innovation that came about…
Descriptors: Morphemes, Language Acquisition, Indo European Languages, Standard Spoken Usage
Aravind, Athulya; de Villiers, Jill; Pace, Amy; Valentine, Hannah; Golinkoff, Roberta; Hirsh-Pasek, Kathy; Iglesias, Aquiles; Wilson, Mary Sweig – Grantee Submission, 2018
Do children learn a new word by tracking co-occurrences between words and referents across multiple instances ("cross-situational learning" models), or is word-learning a "one-track" process, where learners maintain a single hypothesis about the possible referent, which may be verified or falsified in future occurrences…
Descriptors: Young Children, Vocabulary Development, Memory, Retention (Psychology)
Ainsa, Patricia; Rudich, Susan; Fiting, Jessica – Education, 2023
Because of the pandemic and its restrictions on children, a professor and two graduate students employed in early childhood intervention investigated teachers and families in three states to determine if learning and development are affected by inability to physically attend school. The authors also explored if and how academic and…
Descriptors: Early Childhood Teachers, Young Children, Family (Sociological Unit), Pandemics
Jamie J. Jirout; Sierra Eisen; Zoe S. Robertson; Tanya M. Evans – Grantee Submission, 2022
Play is a powerful influence on children's learning and parents can provide opportunities to learn specific content by scaffolding children's play. Parent-child synchrony (i.e., harmony, reciprocity and responsiveness in interactions) is a component of parent-child interactions that is not well characterized in studies of play. We tested whether…
Descriptors: Play, Mothers, Parent Child Relationship, Executive Function
Tolins, Jackson; Namiranian, Neda; Akhtar, Nameera; Fox Tree, Jean E. – First Language, 2017
Children successfully learn words through overhearing others engaged in verbal interactions. The current studies investigated the degree to which four-year-old overhearers are influenced by the response behaviors of addressees and by the interactional pattern of the speakers and addressees. It was found that while addressee responses on their own…
Descriptors: Young Children, Language Acquisition, Vocabulary Development, Dialogs (Language)
Burling, Joseph M.; Yoshida, Hanako – Cognitive Science, 2017
The literature on human and animal learning suggests that individuals attend to and act on cues differently based on the order in which they were learned. Recent studies have proposed that one specific type of learning outcome, the highlighting effect, can serve as a framework for understanding a number of early cognitive milestones. However,…
Descriptors: Early Childhood Education, Young Children, Learning Processes, Bias
Treiman, Rebecca; Kessler, Brett – Language Learning and Development, 2013
Gaining facility with spelling is an important part of becoming a good writer. Here we review recent work on how children learn to spell in alphabetic writing systems. Statistical learning plays an important role in this process. Young children learn about some of the salient graphic characteristics of written texts and attempt to reproduce these…
Descriptors: Alphabets, Writing (Composition), Language Acquisition, Morphology (Languages)
Marschark, Marc, Ed.; Knoors, Harry, Ed. – Oxford University Press, 2020
In recent years, the intersection of cognitive psychology, developmental psychology, and neuroscience with regard to deaf individuals has received increasing attention from a variety of academic and educational audiences. Both research and pedagogy have addressed questions about whether deaf children learn in the same ways that hearing children…
Descriptors: Deafness, Hearing Impairments, Learning Processes, Cognitive Ability
Storkel, Holly L. – Journal of Child Language, 2011
Stoel-Gammon (this issue) states that "from birth to age 2 ; 6, the developing phonological system affects lexical acquisition to a greater degree than lexical factors affect phonological development" (this issue). This conclusion is based on a wealth of data; however, the available data are somewhat limited in scope, focusing on rather holistic…
Descriptors: Child Language, Vocabulary Development, Phonology, Young Children
Kidd, Evan – Developmental Psychology, 2012
This article reports on an individual differences study that investigated the role of implicit statistical learning in the acquisition of syntax in children. One hundred children ages 4 years 5 months through 6 years 11 months completed a test of implicit statistical learning, a test of explicit declarative learning, and standardized tests of…
Descriptors: Learning Processes, Language Acquisition, Syntax, Language Patterns
Yuan, Sylvia Hsin Wei – ProQuest LLC, 2009
Children use syntax as well as observations of events to learn verb meanings. This is known as syntactic bootstrapping. This dissertation investigated the origins and mechanisms of syntactic bootstrapping. Prior evidence suggested that two-year-olds, but not younger children, could use aspects of sentence structure to assign different…
Descriptors: Verbs, Sentence Structure, Sentences, Semantics
Hansen, Mikkel B.; Markman, Ellen M. – Developmental Psychology, 2009
When teaching children part terms, adults frequently outline the relevant part rather than simply point. This pragmatic information very likely helps children interpret the label correctly. But the importance of gestures may not negate the need for default lexical biases such as the whole object assumption and mutual exclusivity. On this view,…
Descriptors: Bilingualism, Language Acquisition, Foreign Countries, Preschool Education
Thom, Emily E.; Sandhofer, Catherine M. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2009
This study experimentally tested the relationship between children's lexicon size and their ability to learn new words within the domain of color. We manipulated the size of 25 20-month-olds' color lexicons by training them with two, four, or six different color words over the course of eight training sessions. We subsequently tested children's…
Descriptors: Color, Training, Vocabulary, Language Acquisition