Publication Date
In 2025 | 23 |
Since 2024 | 120 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 529 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 1205 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 3207 |
Descriptor
Source
Author
Johnson, Scott P. | 21 |
Wagemans, Johan | 17 |
Quinn, Paul C. | 15 |
Smith, Linda B. | 15 |
Maurer, Daphne | 14 |
Rayner, Keith | 14 |
Rose, Susan A. | 14 |
Haith, Marshall M. | 13 |
Humphreys, Glyn W. | 13 |
Bhatt, Ramesh S. | 12 |
Cowan, Nelson | 12 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Education Level
Audience
Researchers | 155 |
Practitioners | 147 |
Teachers | 96 |
Parents | 6 |
Students | 5 |
Administrators | 4 |
Community | 2 |
Counselors | 2 |
Media Staff | 2 |
Support Staff | 2 |
Policymakers | 1 |
More ▼ |
Location
China | 70 |
Canada | 66 |
Australia | 56 |
Turkey | 51 |
United Kingdom | 48 |
Germany | 46 |
Italy | 39 |
United Kingdom (England) | 39 |
Taiwan | 33 |
Netherlands | 31 |
Israel | 29 |
More ▼ |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Elementary and Secondary… | 8 |
No Child Left Behind Act 2001 | 2 |
Copyright Law 1976 | 1 |
Education Consolidation… | 1 |
Elementary and Secondary… | 1 |
Elementary and Secondary… | 1 |
Individuals with Disabilities… | 1 |
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Crookes, Kate; Hayward, William G. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2012
Presenting a face inverted (upside down) disrupts perceptual sensitivity to the spacing between the features. Recently, it has been shown that this disruption is greater for vertical than horizontal changes in eye position. One explanation for this effect proposed that inversion disrupts the processing of long-range (e.g., eye-to-mouth distance)…
Descriptors: Human Body, Visual Perception, Spatial Ability, Change
Jesse, Alexandra; Johnson, Elizabeth K. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2012
Using a referent detection paradigm, we examined whether listeners can determine the object speakers are referring to by using the temporal alignment between the motion speakers impose on objects and their labeling utterances. Stimuli were created by videotaping speakers labeling a novel creature. Without being explicitly instructed to do so,…
Descriptors: Speech, Nonverbal Communication, Suprasegmentals, Time
Burnett Heyes, Stephanie; Zokaei, Nahid; van der Staaij, Irene; Bays, Paul M.; Husain, Masud – Developmental Science, 2012
Visual working memory (VWM) is the facility to hold in mind visual information for brief periods of time. Developmental studies have suggested an increase during childhood in the maximum number of complete items that can simultaneously be stored in VWM. Here, we exploit a recent theoretical and empirical innovation to investigate instead the…
Descriptors: Short Term Memory, Visual Perception, Children, Early Adolescents
Hubbard, Timothy L. – Psychological Bulletin, 2012
White (2012) proposed that kinematic features in a visual percept are matched to stored representations containing information regarding forces (based on prior haptic experience) and that information in the matched, stored representations regarding forces is then incorporated into visual perception. Although some elements of White's (2012) account…
Descriptors: Cues, Motion, Visual Perception, Cognitive Development
Molet, Mikael; Gambet, Boris; Bugallo, Mehdi; Miller, Ralph R. – Learning and Motivation, 2012
The role of context was examined in the selection and integration of independently learned spatial relationships. Using a dynamic 3D virtual environment, participants learned one spatial relationship between landmarks A and B which was established in one virtual context (e.g., A is left of B) and a different spatial relationship which was…
Descriptors: Classroom Techniques, Numeracy, Spatial Ability, Virtual Classrooms
Sims, Chris R.; Jacobs, Robert A.; Knill, David C. – Psychological Review, 2012
Limits in visual working memory (VWM) strongly constrain human performance across many tasks. However, the nature of these limits is not well understood. In this article we develop an ideal observer analysis of human VWM by deriving the expected behavior of an optimally performing but limited-capacity memory system. This analysis is framed around…
Descriptors: Models, Short Term Memory, Visual Stimuli, Task Analysis
Koenderink, Jan; van Doorn, Andrea; Wagemans, Johan – Cognition, 2012
Cartoon-style line drawings contain very condensed information, after all most of the page stays blank. Yet, they constrain the contents of immediate visual awareness to an extraordinary extent. This is true even for drawings that are--though nominally "representational"--not even in central projection. Moreover, the strokes used in a drawing may…
Descriptors: Art Education, Cartoons, Artists, Cues
Most, Tova; Michaelis, Hilit – Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 2012
Purpose: This study aimed to investigate the effect of hearing loss (HL) on emotion-perception ability among young children with and without HL. Method: A total of 26 children 4.0-6.6 years of age with prelingual sensory-neural HL ranging from moderate to profound and 14 children with normal hearing (NH) participated. They were asked to identify…
Descriptors: Young Children, Hearing Impairments, Auditory Perception, Visual Perception
Suavansri, Ketchai; Falchook, Adam D.; Williamson, John B.; Heilman, Kenneth M. – Brain and Cognition, 2012
Background: Pseudoneglect is a normal left sided spatial bias observed with attempted bisections of horizontal lines and a normal upward bias observed with attempted bisections of vertical lines. Horizontal pseudoneglect has been attributed to right hemispheric dominance for the allocation of attention. The goal of this study was to test the…
Descriptors: Brain Hemisphere Functions, Handedness, Spatial Ability, Lateral Dominance
Kemp, Charles; Shafto, Patrick; Tenenbaum, Joshua B. – Cognitive Psychology, 2012
Humans routinely make inductive generalizations about unobserved features of objects. Previous accounts of inductive reasoning often focus on inferences about a single object or feature: accounts of causal reasoning often focus on a single object with one or more unobserved features, and accounts of property induction often focus on a single…
Descriptors: Generalization, Logical Thinking, Inferences, Probability
Gabbard, Carl; Cordova, Alberto – Journal of Genetic Psychology, 2012
In this article, the authors examined the effects of target information presented in different visual fields (lower, upper, central) on estimates of reach via use of motor imagery in children (5-11 years old) and young adults. Results indicated an advantage for estimating reach movements for targets placed in lower visual field (LoVF), with all…
Descriptors: Visual Perception, Psychomotor Skills, Children, Young Adults
Gillam, Barbara J.; Grove, Philip M. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2011
Figure-ground perception is typically described as seeing one surface occluding another. Figure properties, not ground properties, are considered the significant factors. In scenes, however, a near surface will often occlude multiple contours and surfaces, often at different depths, producing alignments that are improbable except under conditions…
Descriptors: Visual Perception, Probability, Geometric Concepts, Visual Stimuli
Van der Haegen, Lise; Brysbaert, Marc – Brain and Language, 2011
Words are processed as units. This is not as evident as it seems, given the division of the human cerebral cortex in two hemispheres and the partial decussation of the optic tract. In two experiments, we investigated what underlies the unity of foveally presented words: A bilateral projection of visual input in foveal vision, or interhemispheric…
Descriptors: Inhibition, Visual Perception, Word Recognition, Experiments
Ross-Sheehy, Shannon; Oakes, Lisa M.; Luck, Steven J. – Developmental Science, 2011
Two experiments examined the hypothesis that developing visual attentional mechanisms influence infants' Visual Short-Term Memory (VSTM) in the context of multiple items. Five- and 10-month-old infants (N = 76) received a change detection task in which arrays of three differently colored squares appeared and disappeared. On each trial one square…
Descriptors: Attention, Infants, Visual Perception, Short Term Memory
Moll, Henrike; Meltzoff, Andrew N. – Child Development, 2011
Previous research has found that children engage in Level 2 visual perspective-taking, that is, the understanding that others may see things in a different way, between 4 and 5 years of age (e.g., J. H. Flavell, B. A. Everett, K. Croft, & E. R. Flavell, 1981). This ability was reexamined in 36-month-olds using color filters. In Experiment 1 (N =…
Descriptors: Perspective Taking, Toddlers, Visual Perception, Theory of Mind