NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Showing 8,431 to 8,445 of 11,083 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Ye, Yun; Connine, Cynthia M. – Language and Cognitive Processes, 1999
Reports the results of three experiments that used vowel and tone monitoring tasks to investigate the role of tone information in processing Mandarin. (Author/VWL)
Descriptors: College Students, Higher Education, Language Processing, Language Research
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Hulstijn, Jan H. – Language Learning & Technology, 2000
Gives a brief characterization of the ways second-language acquisition researchers use the computer to elicit second-language (L2) production data or to record how L2 learners process L2 input. Eight tasks and/or techniques are described. Discusses the use of computer technology in ongoing investigations that pertain to the acquisition of…
Descriptors: Computer Uses in Education, Language Processing, Language Research, Language Skills
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Morton, J. Bruce; Trehub, Sandra E. – Child Development, 2001
Explored in three experiments children's understanding of emotion in speech. Found gradual developmental change from 4-year-olds' focus on content to adult's focus on paralanguage. Children exhibited greater response latencies to utterances with conflicting cues than to those with nonconflicting cues. They accurately labeled affective paralanguage…
Descriptors: Adults, Age Differences, Children, Cognitive Development
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Barnes-Holmes, Dermot; Staunton, Carmel; Whelan, Robert; Barnes-Holmes, Yvonne; Commins, Sean; Walsh, Derek; Stewart, Ian; Smeets, Paul M.; Dymond, Simon – Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 2005
Derived equivalence relations, it has been argued, provide a behavioral model of semantic or symbolic meaning in natural language, and thus equivalence relations should possess properties that are typically associated with semantic relations. The present study sought to test this basic postulate using semantic priming. Across three experiments,…
Descriptors: Semantics, Priming, Language Processing, Reaction Time
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Guasti, Maria Teresa; Chierchia, Gennaro; Crain, Stephen; Foppolo, Francesca; Gualmini, Andrea; Meroni, Luisa – Language and Cognitive Processes, 2005
Noveck (2001) argued that children even as old as 11 do not reliably endorse a scalar interpretation of weak scalar terms ("some", "might", "or") (cf. Braine & Rumain, 1981; Smith, 1980). More recent studies suggest, however, that children's apparent failures may depend on the experimental demands (Papafragou…
Descriptors: Research Methodology, Prerequisites, Young Children, Adults
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Sutherland, Dean; Gillon, Gail T. – Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 2005
Purpose: This study explored the use of assessment tasks to examine underlying phonological representations in preschool children with speech impairment. The study also investigated the association between performance on phonological representation tasks and phonological awareness development. Method: The performance of 9 children (aged 3;09…
Descriptors: Task Analysis, Speech Impairments, Reading Skills, Preschool Children
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Dipper, Lucy T.; Black, Maria; Bryan, Karen L. – Language and Cognitive Processes, 2005
In this paper, we reconsider some of the processes that distinguish production and comprehension. In particular, we discuss the specific forms of thinking involved in each: "thinking for speaking" and "thinking for listening" (Black and Chiat, 2000; Slobin, 1996). We argue that thinking for speaking (or for any form of language output) crucially…
Descriptors: Speech Communication, Linguistic Input, Interaction, Language Impairments
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Van Lancker Sidtis, Diana – International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 2003
Although interest in the language sciences was previously focused on newly created sentences, more recently much attention has turned to the importance of formulaic expressions in normal and disordered communication. Also referred to as formulaic expressions and made up of speech formulas, idioms, expletives, serial and memorized speech, slang,…
Descriptors: Sociolinguistics, Psycholinguistics, Neurology, Sentences
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Snellings, P.; van Gelderen, A.; de Glopper, K. – Language Testing, 2004
Lexical retrieval is an essential subprocess of language production and crucial in fluent writing and speaking. In this study we discuss a new measure of the speed of written lexical retrieval in a second language, the Written Productive Translation Task (WPTT). In contrast to Picture Naming tasks, the WPTT is not restricted to concrete nouns and…
Descriptors: Second Languages;Second Language Learning, Language Tests, Written Language, Vocabulary
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Smith, Mark; Wheeldon, Linda – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2004
In 4 experiments the authors used a variant of the picture-word interference paradigm to investigate whether there is a temporal overlap in the activation of words during sentence production and whether there is a flow of semantic and phonological information between them. Experiments 1 and 2 demonstrate that 2 semantically related nouns produce…
Descriptors: Semantics, Sentences, Nouns, Speech
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Criss, Amy H.; Shiffrin, Richard M. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2004
In studies of episodic recognition memory, low-frequency words (LF) have higher hit rates (HR) and lower false alarm rates (FAR) than do high-frequency words (HF), which is known as the mirror pattern. A few findings have suggested that requiring a task at study may reduce or eliminate the LF-HR advantage without altering the LF-FAR effect. Other…
Descriptors: Word Frequency, Language Processing, Recognition (Psychology), Memory
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Avraamides, Marios N.; Loomis, Jack M.; Klatzky, Roberta L.; Golledge, Reginald G. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2004
Past research (e.g., J. M. Loomis, Y. Lippa, R. L. Klatzky, & R. G. Golledge, 2002) has indicated that spatial representations derived from spatial language can function equivalently to those derived from perception. The authors tested functional equivalence for reporting spatial relations that were not explicitly stated during learning.…
Descriptors: Vision, Visual Perception, Spatial Ability, Cognitive Processes
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Moscoso del Prado Martin, Fermin; Bertram, Raymond; Haikio, Tuomo; Schreuder, Robert; Baayen, R. Harald – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2004
Finnish has a very productive morphology in which a stem can give rise to several thousand words. This study presents a visual lexical decision experiment addressing the processing consequences of the huge productivity of Finnish morphology. The authors observed that in Finnish words with larger morphological families elicited shorter response…
Descriptors: Indo European Languages, Morphology (Languages), Semitic Languages, Semantics
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Tijms, Jurgen – Journal of Research in Reading, 2004
This study examines whether two frequently reported causes of dyslexia, phonological processing problems and verbal memory impairments, represent a double-deficit or whether they are two expressions of the same deficit. Two-hundred-and-sixty-seven Dutch children aged 10-14 with dyslexia completed a list-learning task and several phonological…
Descriptors: Memory, Spelling, Dyslexia, Phonology
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Shatzman, Keren B.; Schiller, Niels O. – Brain and Language, 2004
Models of speech production disagree on whether or not homonyms have a shared word-form representation. To investigate this issue, a picture-naming experiment was carried out using Dutch homonyms of which both meanings could be presented as a picture. Naming latencies for the low-frequency meanings of homonyms were slower than for those of the…
Descriptors: Word Frequency, Hypothesis Testing, Models, Speech
Pages: 1  |  ...  |  559  |  560  |  561  |  562  |  563  |  564  |  565  |  566  |  567  |  ...  |  739